Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 03 July 2019
Sec. Motor Neuroscience
Volume 13 - 2019 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2019.00207

Explicit Control of Step Timing During Split-Belt Walking Reveals Interdependent Recalibration of Movements in Space and Time

Marcela Gonzalez-Rubio Nicolas F. Velasquez Gelsy Torres-Oviedo*
  • Department of Bioengineering, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA, United States

Split-belt treadmills that move the legs at different speeds are thought to update internal representations of the environment, such that this novel condition generates a new locomotor pattern with distinct spatio-temporal features compared to those of regular walking. It is unclear the degree to which such recalibration of movements in the spatial and temporal domains is interdependent. In this study, we explicitly altered subjects' limb motion in either space or time during split-belt walking to determine its impact on the adaptation of the other domain. Interestingly, we observed that motor adaptation in the spatial domain was susceptible to altering the temporal domain, whereas motor adaptation in the temporal domain was resilient to modifying the spatial domain. This non-reciprocal relation suggests a hierarchical organization such that the control of timing in locomotion has an effect on the control of limb position. This is of translational interest because clinical populations often have a greater deficit in one domain compared to the other. Our results suggest that explicit changes to temporal deficits cannot occur without modifying the spatial control of the limb.

1. Introduction

We are constantly adapting our movements to demands imposed by changes in the environment or our body. In walking, this requires the adaptation of spatial and temporal gait features to control “where” and “when” we step, respectively. Particularly, in split-belt walking when one leg moves faster than the other, it has been observed that subjects minimize spatial and temporal asymmetries by adopting motor patterns specific to the split environment (e.g., Malone et al., 2012). It is thought that this is achieved by updating internal representations of the treadmill for the control of the limb in space and time (Malone et al., 2012). There is a clinical interest in understanding the interdependence in the control of these two aspects of movement because pathological gait often has a greater deficiency in one domain compared to the other (Malone and Bastian, 2014; Finley et al., 2015). Thus, there is a translational interest to determine if spatial and temporal asymmetries in clinical populations can be targeted and treated independently.

Ample evidence supports that the adaptation, and hence control, of spatial and temporal gait features is dissociable. Notably, studies have shown that inter-limb measures, such as step timing (temporal) and step position (spatial) adapt at different rates (Malone and Bastian, 2010; Sombric et al., 2017), they exhibit different generalization patterns (Torres-Oviedo and Bastian, 2010), and follow distinct adaptation dynamics throughout development (Vasudevan et al., 2011; Patrick et al., 2014) or healthy aging (Sombric et al., 2017). In addition, several behavioral studies show that subjects' adjustment of spatial metrics can be altered (Malone and Bastian, 2010; Malone et al., 2012; Long et al., 2016) without modifying the adaptation of temporal gait features. However, the opposite has not been demonstrated. For example, altering intra-limb measures (i.e., characterizing single leg motion) of timing, such as stance time duration (Afzal et al., 2015; Krishnan et al., 2016) also leads to changes in intra-limb spatial features, such as stride lengths. In sum, the spatial and temporal control of the limb is thought to be dissociable, but it remains unclear if the adaptation of internal representations of timing can be altered and what is the impact of such manipulation in the temporal domain on the spatial control of the limb.

In this study we aimed to determine the interdependence between the spatial and temporal control of the limbs during walking, particularly of inter-limb parameters characterizing bipedal coordination. We hypothesized that spatial and temporal inter-limb features are adapted independently based on previous studies demonstrating their dissociation. To test this hypothesis, subjects walked on a split-belt treadmill, which requires the adaptation of spatial and temporal inter-limb coordination. We further altered subjects' movements during split-belt walking by either instructing them “where” (spatial feedback) or “when” (temporal feedback) to take a step. We contrasted the impact of explicitly manipulating movements in one domain on the adaptation of the other domain to determine their interdependence.

2. Materials and Methods

We recruited twenty-one healthy young subjects (13 women, 8 men, mean age 24.69 ± 4 years) to voluntarily participate in this study. Subjects were randomly assigned to three groups (n = 7, each): (1) control, (2) spatial feedback, (3) temporal feedback to determine if explicitly altering the limb motion on either the spatial or the temporal domain with visual feedback during split-belt walking had an impact on the adaptation of the other domain (Figure 1A). Notably, if the control of these two domains was dissociable, altering one would not have an effect on the other. Alternatively, if they were interdependent, modifying the adaptation of one domain not only would have an effect on the targeted domain, but will also alter the other one. The protocol was approved by the Institutional Review Board of the University of Pittsburgh and all subjects gave informed consent prior to testing.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Expected outcomes, paradigm and feedback visualization. (A) Expected outcomes for dissociable and interdependent internal representations of space and time. If dissociable, the feedback manipulation will only affect the targeted domain without changing the other domain. For example, spatial feedback (indicated with blue outline) would alter spatial features (S) of the motor pattern while temporal ones (T) remain invariant. On the other hand, if the domains are interdependent, feedback manipulation of one domain will also alter the other domain. For example, spatial feedback modifying spatial features of the motor pattern would also change temporal ones. (B) Split-belt walking paradigm used in all groups. Dashed lines separate the different experimental phases. All groups experienced the same number of strides during each phase (Baseline: 150, Familiarization: 150, Adaptation: 600, and Post-adaptation: 450). The two belts moved at the same speed (0.75m/s) during the Baseline and Familiarization phases. Only subjects in the feedback groups walked while observing their movements on a TV screen placed directly in front of them (Feedback On) during the familiarization phase. The feedback to these groups was also given during the Adaptation phase (gray shaded area) during which one belt (fast belt) moved at 1m/s and the other one (slow belt) moved at 0.5m/s. Finally, during Post-adaptation subjects walked again with the two belts moving at the same speed (0.75m/s). (C,D) Visual feedback schematic. Schematic of the legs in the top row illustrate the step position (e.g., αf and αs) and step time (e.g., ts), which were the walking features used in the spatial and temporal feedback tasks, respectively. Bottom rows in (C,D) illustrate the screen shots observed by individuals in the spatial feedback group (C) or in the temporal feedback group (D). Blue rectangles indicated the target step position or step time value that subjects had to achieve with each leg. These rectangles turned green when subjects met the desired step position or step time values and red when they did not. Yellow lines indicated either the step position value (C) or the step time value (D) at heel strike (HS) when taking a step with the right or left leg (e.g., left leg's step position is shown in the screen shot #1). In the example shown, the step position was correct for the right leg but not for the left leg. The light gray progression bars showed in real-time either the distance from the ankle to the hip markers as subjects swing the leg forward (C) or the time that the subject had spent on the standing leg since it hit the ground (D).

2.1. Experimental Protocol

All subjects walked on a split-belt treadmill during four experimental phases: Baseline, Familiarization, Adaptation, and Post-adaptation. The speed for each belt during these phases is shown in Figure 1B. This speed profile enabled individuals to walk at an averaged speed of 0.75 m/s throughout the experiment. In the Baseline phase, individuals walked with the two belts moving at the same speed of 0.75 m/s for 150 strides (~ 3 min). Recordings from these phase were used as the reference gait for every individual. In the Familiarization phase, all participants also walked at 0.75 m/s for 150 strides, but only subjects in the feedback groups received the same visual feedback that they were going to experience during the subsequent Adaptation phase. This was done to allow feedback groups to become habituated to use the provided visual feedback to control either spatial (spatial feedback group) or temporal (temporal feedback group) gait features. In the Adaptation phase, the belts were moved at a 2:1 ratio (1:0.5 m/s) for 600 strides (~13 min). We selected these specific belt speeds because other studies have indicated that they induce robust sensorimotor adaptation (Reisman et al., 2005; Mawase et al., 2014; Sombric et al., 2017; Vervoort et al., 2019) and we observed in pilot tests that subjects with visual feedback at these speeds could successfully modify the spatial and temporal gait features of interest. The self-reported dominant leg walked on the fast belt. In the Post-adaptation phase, all individuals walked with both belts moving at 0.75 m/s for 450 strides (~10 min). This phase was used to quantify gait changes following the Adaptation phase. The treadmill belts were stopped at the end of each experimental phase. A handrail was placed in front of the treadmill for safety purposes, but individuals did not hold it while walking. A custom-built divider was placed in the middle of the treadmill during the entire experimental protocol to prevent subjects from stepping on the same belt with both legs. Subjects also wore a safety harness (SoloStep, SD) that did not interfere with their walking (no body weight support).

We tested three groups: (1) control group, (2) spatial feedback group, (3) temporal feedback group. The control group was asked to “just walk” without any specific feedback on subjects' movements. Each subject in the spatial or temporal feedback groups was instructed to either maintain his/her averaged baseline step position (spatial feedback group) or averaged baseline step time (temporal feedback group) when the feedback was on. Step position was defined as the sagittal distance between the leading leg's ankle to the hip at heel strike (Figure 1C). Step time was defined as the time period from heel strike (i.e., foot landing) of one leg to heel strike of the other leg (Figure 1D). We chose to manipulate step position and step time for consistency with other studies (Malone et al., 2012; Long et al., 2016) and because these parameters are adjusted during split-belt walking to reduce spatial and temporal inter-limb asymmetries, respectively (Malone et al., 2012). Panels C and D in Figure 1 show sample screen shots of the visual feedback observed by each group on a screen placed in front of them. More specifically, we permanently displayed either spatial or temporal targets (blue rectangles) indicating the averaged step position (spatial feedback group) or averaged step time (temporal feedback group) across legs during baseline walking. These targets turned green when subjects achieved the targeted baseline values and they turned red when they did not. A tolerance of ±0.75 and ±1.25% of the baseline value was given to subjects in the spatial and temporal feedback groups, respectively. Yellow lines indicated the actual step position and step time for each leg at every step. Thus, subjects could appreciate how far they were from the targeted spatial or temporal value at every step.

2.2. Data Collection

Kinetic and kinematic data were collected to quantify subjects' gait. Kinematic data was collected at 100 Hz with a motion capture system (VICON motion systems, Oxford, UK). Passive reflective markers were placed bilaterally on bony landmarks at the ankle (malleolus) and the hip (greater trochanter). Kinetic data was collected at 1,000 Hz with the instrumented split-belt treadmill (Bertec, OH). The normal ground reaction force (Fz) was used to detect when the foot landed (i.e., heel strike) or was lifted off (i.e., toe off). A threshold of 10 N was used for detecting heel strikes and toe offs for data analysis, whereas a threshold of 30 N was used for counting strides in real-time.

2.3. Data Analysis

2.3.1. Gait Parameters

We computed six gait parameters previously used (Malone et al., 2012) to quantify the adaptation of spatial and temporal control of the limb during split-belt walking: Sout, Tout, SA, TA, SnA, and TnA. We used Sout and Tout because our feedback was designed to directly alter these metrics. For example, subjects in the spatial feedback group were given feedback to maintain the same baseline step position in both legs. Sout is, therefore, a good metric of performance for the spatial feedback group since it quantifies the difference in step positions, αf and αs, when taking a step with the fast and slow leg, respectively. Formally expressed:

Sout=αf-αsαf+αs    (1)

αi is a length measurement that indicates the position of the ankle marker relative to the hip marker at heel strike. The subscript i can be either f or s for the leg that is on the fast belt or slow belt, respectively. By convention, Sout is positive when the fast leg's foot lands farther away from the body when taking a step than the slow leg's one (i.e., αf > αs). Sout is zero during baseline and subjects in the feedback group were instructed to maintain this value during split-belt walking.

Similarly, subjects in the temporal feedback group were given feedback to maintain the same baseline step times in both legs. Tout is, therefore, a good metric of performance for the temporal feedback group since it quantifies the difference in step times, ts and tf. Step time ts is defined as the time interval to take a step on the slow belt (i.e., duration from heel strike on the fast belt to the subsequent heel strike on the slow belt) and vice versa for ts. Formally expressed:

Tout=ts-tfts+tf=ts-tfTstride    (2)

Where Tstride is the stride time (i.e., time interval between two consecutive heel strikes with the same leg). By convention, Tout is positive when the slow leg's step time is longer that the fast leg's one. Tout is zero during baseline and subjects in the feedback group were instructed to maintain this value during split-belt walking. It has been previously shown that Sout and Tout are adapted during split-belt walking to minimize spatial and temporal baseline asymmetries defined as SA and TA, respectively (Malone et al., 2012). Therefore, we also quantified SA and TA because these are adaptive parameters (Reisman et al., 2005; Malone and Bastian, 2010; Malone et al., 2012) that could be indirectly altered by our spatial and temporal feedback even if subjects in these groups were not explicitly instructed to modify them.

SA quantifies differences between the legs in where they oscillate with respect to the body. The oscillation of each leg was computed as the ratio between two distances: step position (α) and stride length (γ) (i.e., anterior-posterior distance from foot position at heel strike to ipsilateral foot position at toe off). Thus, SA (legs' orientation asymmetry) was computed as the difference between these ratios when taking a step with the slow leg (i.e., slow leg leading) vs. the fast leg (see Equation 3).

SA=αsγs-αfγf    (3)

In the temporal domain, TA quantified the difference in double support times (i.e., period during which both legs are on the ground) when taking a step with the fast leg (DSs) or slow leg (DSf), respectively (see Equation 4). In other words, DSs is defined as the time from fast heel strike to slow toe off and DSf as the time from slow heel strike to fast toe off.

TA=DSs-DSf    (4)

Lastly, we computed gait parameters defined as SnA and TnA, to test the specificity of our feedback. Namely, it has been previously observed that these parameters do not change as subjects walk in the split-belt environment (Reisman et al., 2005; Malone et al., 2012; Yokoyama et al., 2018). Thus, these measures are thought to simply reflect the speed difference between the legs, and hence, we expected that our feedback would not alter them. Specifically, SnA quantifies the difference between the fast and slow leg's ranges of motion γf and γs. Formally expressed as:

SnA=γf-γsγf+γs    (5)

The non-adaptive measure in the temporal domain TnA quantifies the difference between the slow and fast leg's stance time durations (which is defined as the interval when the foot is in contact with the ground), which we labeled as STs and STf, respectively. Formally expressed as:

TnA=STs-STfTstride    (6)

2.3.2. Outcome Measures

We computed steady state and after-effects to respectively characterize the adaptation and recalibration of walking in the spatial and temporal domains. Both of these outcome measures were computed for each gait parameter described in the previous section. Steady state was used to characterize the spatial and temporal features of the adapted motor pattern once subjects reached a plateau during split-belt walking. Steady state was computed as the averaged of the last 40 strides during the Adaptation phase, except for the very last 5 strides to exclude transient steps when subjects were told to hold on to the handrail prior to stopping the treadmill. After-effects were used to characterize the recalibration of subjects' internal representation of the environment (Roemmich and Bastian, 2015) leading to gait changes that were sustained following split-belt walking compared to baseline spatial and temporal gait features. After-effects were computed as the averaged value for each gait parameter over the first thirty strides of post-adaptation. We used 30 strides, rather than only the initial 1–5 strides, because we were interested in characterizing long lasting after-effects (Long et al., 2015; Roemmich and Bastian, 2015; Mawase et al., 2017). We removed baseline biases from both measures by subtracting the baseline values for each gait parameter averaged over the last 40 strides during baseline (minus the very last transient 5 strides). This was done to exclude individual biases before aggregating subjects' outcome measures in every group.

2.4. Statistical Analysis

We performed separate two-way repeated measures ANOVAs (factors: group and epoch) comparing the control group to either the temporal or spatial feedback groups. This was done to determine the effect of experimentally altering either spatial or temporal measures during split-belt walking on outcome measures in both domains. When main effects of group or epoch were found (p < 0.05), we used Fisher's LSD post-hoc testing to assess if main effects were driven by differences between the control group and feedback group in either domain. We applied a Bonferroni correction to account for 2 comparisons of interest resulting in a significance level set to α = 0.025. We selected to do our analysis with unbiased data (i.e., subject-specific baseline bias removed) to reduce inter-subject variability due to distinct baseline biases and focus on group effects due to the distinct experimental manipulations. Lastly, we performed independent sample t-tests to determine if steady state or after-effects were significantly different from baseline. We applied Bonferroni corrections to account for four comparisons of interest (baseline vs. steady state and baseline vs. after-effects for each of the experimentally targeted Sout and Tout parameters) setting the significance level to α = 0.0125. For all other parameters, we set the significance level to α = 0.025 to account for only two comparisons of interest (baseline vs. after-effects in the spatial and temporal domains). This was done since we were primarily interested in the impact of the experimental manipulation on the after-effects of the parameters that were not explicitly targeted with the visual feedback.

3. Results

3.1. Confirmation of Results Supporting Dissociable Representation of Spatial and Temporal Walking Features

Spatial and temporal gait features adapted and recalibrated independently when feedback was used to alter the spatial control of the limb. This is indicated by the group differences qualitatively observed in the Sout's time courses during Adaptation and Post-adaptation (left panel in Figures 2A,B, respectively) contrasting the overlapping time courses of Tout in the control group (red trace) and spatial feedback group (blue trace) (right panel in Figures 2A,B). Accordingly, we found a significant group effect on Sout (p = 0.0039), but not a group (p = 0.3748) or group by epoch interaction effect on Tout (p = 0.2293). Post-hoc analysis indicated that the spatial feedback reduced the steady state of Sout relative to the control group (SS : p = 0.0021); such that the steady state values reached by the spatial feedback group were not significantly different from zero (p = 0.0481), whereas those of the control group differed from zero (p = 0.0004). This indicated that individuals in the spatial feedback group were able to maintain their baseline Sout values with the visual feedback on this metric. In contrast, the steady state values of Tout were significantly different from zero in both groups (control group: p < 0.0001; spatial feedback group: p = 0.0004). The dissociation between spatial and temporal control was also shown by the after-effects of Sout and Tout in the control vs. spatial feedback groups (Figure 2B). Post-hoc analysis indicated that the spatial feedback group had reduced after-effects of Sout compared to the control group (SS : p = 0.0159) and that only the control group had after-effects different from zero (control group: p = 0.0003; spatial feedback group: p = 0.0164). Conversely, Tout was once again not qualitatively different between the groups and the after-effects were non-significantly different from zero on either group (control group: p = 0.4235; spatial feedback group: p = 0.1023). In sum, spatial feedback had a domain-specific effect: it altered the adaptation and recalibration of Sout (targeted spatial parameter) without modifying the adaptation and aftereffects of step time (Tout).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Adaptation and post-adaptation of the parameters Sout (targeted) and Tout in the spatial feedback and control groups. Stride-by-stride time courses show the effect of altering step positions in the adaptation (A) and post-adaptation (B) of Sout and Tout. Each data point in the time courses represents the average of five consecutive strides and shaded areas around the data points represent the standard errors. Bar plots indicate the mean average behavior in the epochs of interest (indicated with the black rectangles), gray dots indicate values for individual subjects, and vertical black lines are standard errors. Horizontal lines between bars illustrate significant differences between groups (p < 0.025). (A) Steady state values of Sout and Tout: we found a significant group difference in Sout's steady state. Colored asterisks indicate that the mean steady state for that group is significantly different from zero (p < 0.0125). (B) After-effect values of Sout and Tout: we found a significant group difference in Sout's after-effects. Colored asterisks indicate that the mean after-effect for that group is significantly different from zero (p < 0.0125).

The dissociation in adaptation and recalibration of spatial and temporal representations of walking was also supported by the analysis of spatial and temporal features known to be adapted by the split-belt task, but not directly targeted by our feedback. Namely, the spatial feedback also modified the Adaptation and Post-adaptation time courses of the legs' orientation asymmetry quantified by SA, which is expected given its relation to Sout. Note that the time courses of SA for the spatial feedback group (blue trace) and control group (red trace) do not overlap during Adaptation and Post-adaptation (left panel Figures 3A,B). In contrast, the time courses of double support asymmetry (TA) were not altered by the spatial feedback, as shown by the overlap of TA values during Adaptation and Post-adaptation of the temporal feedback and control groups (right panel Figures 3A,B). Consistently, we found a significant group effect in SA (p = 0.0091) and a non-significant group (p = 0.8679) or group by epoch interaction (p = 0.2229) in TA. Post-hoc analyses revealed that between group differences in SA were driven by the significantly different SA's steady state (SSA : p = 0.0177) and trending differences in SA's after-effects (SSA : p = 0.0358); such that after-effects were significant in the control group (p = 0.0009) but not in the spatial feedback group (p = 0.0542). Conversely, after-effects in double support asymmetry (TA) were significantly different from zero in all groups (control group:p = 0.0044; spatial feedback group:p = 0.0007). These results reiterated that changes in the spatial domain did not modify the temporal control of the limb in the temporal domain, replicating previous findings (Malone et al., 2012; Long et al., 2016).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Adaptation and post-adaptation for the adaptive but non-targeted parameters SA (leg orientation asymmetry) and TA (double support time asymmetry) in the spatial feedback and control groups. Stride-by-stride time courses show the effect of altering the step positions in the adaptation (A) and post-adaptation (B) of SA and TA. Each data point in the time courses represents the average of five consecutive strides and shaded areas around the data points represent the standard errors. Bar plots indicate the mean average behavior in the epochs of interest (indicated with the black rectangles), the gray dots indicate values for individual subjects, and vertical black lines are standard errors. Horizontal lines between bars illustrate significant differences between groups (p < 0.025). We found a significant group effect in SA. (A) Steady States for SA and TA: the significant group effect on SA was driven by differences between the spatial feedback and control group in the non-targeted spatial motor output (adaptive motor output). (B) After-Effects values of SA and TA: we found significant group differences in SA. Colored asterisks indicate after-effect values are significantly different from zero (p < 0.025) according to post-hoc analysis.

3.2. New Evidence for Interdependent Representations of Spatial and Temporal Walking Features

Interestingly, we found that spatial and temporal gait features were not independent in their adaptation and recalibration when feedback was used to alter the temporal control of the limb. This is indicated by the qualitative differences between the time courses of Tout and Sout during the Adaptation (Figure 4A) and Post-adaptation phases (Figure 4B). Namely, the control group (red traces) and temporal feedback group (yellow traces) are different in both spatial and temporal parameters. Consistently, we found a significant group effect on Sout (p = 0.0005) and Tout (p = 0.0034). Post-hoc analyses revealed that the Tout's steady state was significantly different from zero in the control (p = 0.0004) and temporal feedback group (p = 0.0092). Thus, subjects in the temporal feedback group did not fully maintained the baseline values of Tout, even if they were able to use the visual feedback to significantly reduce the Tout steady state during split-belt walking relative to the control group (TT : p < 0.0001). While the temporal feedback group was designed to alter Tout, we did not anticipate a reduction in the Sout's steady state relative to the control group (TS : p = 0.0027) because this parameter was not directly targeted by the feedback. The interdependence between spatial and temporal domains was also shown by the analysis of after-effects in Post-adaptation (Figure 4B). Post-hoc analyses indicated that temporal feedback did not change the recalibration of Tout (TT : p = 0.4663), but altered the recalibration of Sout (TS : p = 0.0010). The non-significant effect on the recalibration of Tout was expected given that after-effects in this parameter are very short lived resulting in Tout after-effect values that are non-significantly different from zero (control group: p = 0.4235; temporal feedback group: p = 0.8550). In contrast, both groups had after-effects in Sout that were significantly different from zero (control group: p = 0.0003; temporal feedback group: p = 0.0021), but they were unexpectedly smaller in the temporal feedback group compared to the control group. In sum, the temporal feedback impact on adaptation and recalibration of Sout (spatial parameter) indicated an interdependence between the spatial and temporal control of the limb.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Adaptation and post-adaptation of the parameters Sout and Tout (targeted) in the temporal feedback and control groups. Stride-by-stride time courses show the effect of altering step times in the adaptation (A) and post-adaptation (B) of Sout and Tout. Each data point in the time courses represents the average of five consecutive strides and shaded areas around the data points represent the standard errors. Bar plots indicate the mean average behavior in the epochs of interest (indicated with the black rectangles), the gray dots indicate values for individual subjects, and vertical black lines are standard errors. Horizontal lines between bars illustrate significant differences between groups (p < 0.025). There was a significant group effect on Sout and Tout. (A) Steady States values of Tout and Sout: we found significant group differences in Sout's and Tout's steady state. Colored asterisks indicate that the mean steady state for that group is significantly different from zero (p < 0.0125). (B) After-effect values of Tout and Sout: we found a significant group difference in Sout's after-effects. Colored asterisks indicate that the mean after-effect for that group is significantly different from zero (p < 0.0125).

The possible interdependence in space and time was further supported by the analysis of spatial and temporal features known to be adapted by the split-belt task, but not directly targeted by our feedback. Namely, the temporal feedback also modified the Adaptation and Post-adaptation time courses of the legs' orientation asymmetry, quantified by SA, which is a spatial measure related to step position. Note that the time courses of SA for the temporal feedback group (yellow trace) and control group (red trace) do not overlap during Adaptation and Post-adaptation (left panel Figures 5A,B). In contrast, the time courses of double support asymmetry (TA) were not altered by the temporal feedback, as shown by the overlap of TA values during Adaptation and Post-adaptation of the temporal feedback and control groups (right panel Figures 5A,B). Consistently, we found a group effect in SA (p = 0.0029) and a non-significant group (p = 0.8151) or group by epoch interaction (p = 0.3189) in TA. post-hoc analyses revealed that these effects were driven by group differences in SA's steady state (TSA : p = 0.0138) and SA's after-effects (TSA : p = 0.0163). Surprisingly, we did not find differences on TA's steady state and after-effects, which we expected given the relation between TA and the temporal measure (Tout) directly altered with the temporal feedback. Thus, after-effects in SA and TA were significantly different from zero in all groups (control group: SA : p = 0.0009 and TA : p = 0.0044; temporal feedback group: SA : p = 0.0080 and TA : p = 0.0009), but only those of SA were reduced in the temporal feedback group compared to controls. In sum, these results indicate that temporal feedback did not have a ubiquitous effect in all gait parameters, but it did alter the adaptation and recalibration of the legs' orientation, which also characterizes the spatial control of the limb in locomotion.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Adaptation and post-adaptation for the adaptive but non-targeted parameters SA (leg orientation asymmetry) and TA (double support time asymmetry) in the temporal feedback and control groups. Stride-by-stride time courses show the effect of altering step times in the adaptation (A) and post-adaptation (B) of SA and TA. Each data point in the time courses represents the average of five consecutive strides and shaded areas around the data points represent the standard errors. Bar plots indicate the mean average behavior in the epochs of interest (indicated with the black rectangles), the gray dots indicate values for individual subjects, and vertical black lines are standard errors. Horizontal lines between bars illustrate significant differences between groups (p < 0.025). There was a significant group effect in SA, but no in TA. (A) Steady State values of TA and SA: the significant group effect on SA was driven by differences between the temporal feedback and control group in the non-targeted spatial motor output (adaptive motor output). (B) After-effects of TA and SA: we found a significant group difference in SA. Colored asterisks indicate after-effect values are significantly different from zero (p < 0.025) according to post-hoc analysis.

3.3. Temporal Feedback Modified the Split-Belt Task to a Greater Extent Than the Spatial Feedback

Surprisingly, temporal feedback altered the difference in stance times between the legs (TnA), whereas the spatial feedback did not. This was unexpected given previous literature indicating that SnA and TnA do not change as subjects walk in the split-belt environment (Reisman et al., 2005; Malone et al., 2012; Yokoyama et al., 2018). Thus, we anticipated that either type of feedback (spatial or temporal) would not alter these “non-adaptive” gait features. Qualitatively, we observed that this was the case for the spatial (SnA), but not for the temporal (TnA) “non-adaptive” parameter (Figure 6A). Note that SnA has the same time course for both groups, whereas TnA has a different time course for the control group (red trace) and the temporal feedback group (yellow trace). Consistently, we found a significant group effect (p = 0.0030) and group by epoch interaction (p = 0.0047) in TnA, whereas a non-significant group (p = 0.3860) or group by epoch interaction effect (p = 0.3719) in SnA. Post-hoc analysis revealed that the temporal feedback group reached a significantly lower steady state when compared to the control group (TTnA : p < 0.0001). Conversely, the spatial feedback group exhibited the non-adaptive behavior of these parameters SnA and TnA that we anticipated. Namely, the time courses of SnA (Figure 6B, left panel) and TnA (Figure 6B, right panel) were overlapping in these two groups. This similarity is substantiated by the non-significant group effect (SnA : p = 0.2338 and TnA : p = 0.3002) or group by epoch interaction (SnA : p = 0.7452 and TnA : p = 0.8163) in the non-adaptive spatial and temporal parameter. In sum, feedback modifying the adaptation of spatial and temporal gait features had a distinct effect on “non-adaptive” temporal parameters thought to only depend on the speed difference between the legs in the split-belt task.

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Adaptation of SnA and TnA measures that are non-adaptive and non-targeted parameters in temporal feedback and control group (A) and spatial feedback and control group (B). Stride-by-stride time courses show the effect of altering the step times or step positions on “non-adaptive” temporal and spatial measures (SnA and TnA) during adaptation. Each data point in the time courses represents the average of five consecutive strides and shaded areas around the data points represent the standard errors. Bar plots indicate the mean average behavior in the epochs of interest (indicated with the black rectangles), the gray dots indicate values for individual subjects, and vertical black lines are standard errors. Horizontal lines between bars illustrate significant differences between groups (p < 0.025). (A) Steady State values of TnA and SnA: we found a significant group effect and group by epoch interaction driven by differences between the temporal feedback and control group in the non-targeted temporal motor output (adaptive motor output). (B) Steady State values of SnA and TnA: we did not find a significant group effect or group by epoch interaction for the spatial feedback and control group in the parameters of interest.

4. Discussion

4.1. Summary

Our study confirms previous results suggesting that there are internal representations of space and time for predictive control of movement. We replicated previous results showing that altering the recalibration in the spatial domain does not impact the temporal domain. However, we also observed that the opposite was not true. That is, explicitly reducing the recalibration in the temporal domain altered movement control in space, suggesting some level of interdependence between these two domains. Interestingly, double support asymmetry was consistently corrected across the distinct spatio-temporal perturbations that subjects experienced, whereas spatial asymmetries were not. This indicates that correcting asymmetries in space and time is prioritized differently by the motor system. Our results are of translational interest because clinical populations often have greater deficits in either the spatial or the temporal control of the limb and our findings suggest that they may not be treated in isolation.

4.2. Separate Representations for Predictive Control of Movements in Space and Time

We find that adaptation of movements to a novel walking situation results in the recalibration of internal representations for predictive control of locomotion; which are expressed as robust after-effects in temporal and spatial movement features. This is consistent with the idea that the motor system forms internal representations of space (Marigold and Drew, 2017) and time (Drew and Marigold, 2015; Avraham et al., 2017; Breska and Ivry, 2018) for predictive motor control. Several behavioral studies suggest separate recalibration of these internal representations of space and time in locomotion because spatial and timing measures exhibit different adaptation rates in the mature motor system (Malone and Bastian, 2010; Darmohray et al., 2019) throughout development (Vasudevan et al., 2011; Patrick et al., 2014) or healthy aging (Sombric et al., 2017). Spatial and temporal recalibration also have distinct generalization patterns across walking environments (Torres-Oviedo and Bastian, 2010; Mariscal et al., 2018) and most importantly, altering the adaptation of spatial features does not modify the adaptation and recalibration of temporal ones, as shown by us and others (Malone et al., 2012; Long et al., 2016). This idea of separate representations of space and time in locomotion is also supported by clinical and neurophysiological studies indicating that different neural structures might contribute to the control (Lafreniere-Roula and McCrea, 2005; Rybak et al., 2006) and adaptation (Choi et al., 2009; Vasudevan et al., 2011; Statton et al., 2018) of the spatial and temporal control of the limb in locomotion.

4.3. Hierarchic Control of Timing Leads to Interdependent Adaptation of Movements in Space and Time

Nonetheless, we also found that explicit control of step timing modifies the adaptation and recalibration of movements in space. This result directly contradicts the dissociable adaptation of spatial and temporal features upon explicitly modifying the adaptation of step position (spatial parameter) (Malone et al., 2012; Long et al., 2016). We find two possible explanations to reconcile these findings. First, there might be a hierarchical relationship between the spatial and temporal control of the limb, such that timing cannot be manipulated without obstructing the adaptation of spatial features. We believe that this type of hierarchical organization is not exclusive to explicit control, but it is also applicable to implicit control of the limb in space and time. This is supported by a recent study indicating that lesions to interpose cerebellar nuclei altering the adaptation of double support asymmetry (temporal parameter) also reduced the after-effects of spatial features (Darmohray et al., 2019), whereas the recalibration of spatial features can be halted without modifying the temporal ones (Darmohray et al., 2019). Future studies are needed to determine if similar results would be observed in human bipedal locomotion. This type of hierarchical organization suggests that the execution of spatial and temporal control of the limb can be encoded by separate interneuronal networks (Lafreniere-Roula and McCrea, 2005; Rybak et al., 2006), but the volitional recruitment of those networks cannot occur in isolation. Second, it is possible that the observed interdependence arose as a byproduct of how we tested it. Namely, it remains an open question if our findings result from altering step time, or similar interdependence would be observed if we had manipulated other temporal measures, such as double support asymmetry. More specifically, our feedback on step time inadvertently reduced the stance time asymmetry associated to split-belt walking. The stance time asymmetry is thought to be critical for forcing subjects to adjust their gait during split-belt walking (Reisman et al., 2005). Therefore, subjects in the temporal feedback group might have reduced the adaptation of spatial parameters because the “perturbation” inducing their update was reduced. In sum, future work is needed to determine the generality of temporal measures influencing spatial ones, however our study provides initial evidence for interdependence.

4.4. Relevance of Double Support Symmetry Over Spatial Asymmetries

We demonstrated that double support symmetry (i.e., TA) is recovered in all groups, regardless of the task. This is in accordance with multiple observations that individuals consistently reduce double support asymmetries induced by split-belt walking since very early age (Patrick et al., 2014) or after lesions to cerebral (Reisman et al., 2007) or cerebellar regions (Vasudevan et al., 2011). Only children with hemispherectomies, where half of the cerebrum is missing, do not correct double support asymmetry when this is augmented (Choi et al., 2009). The adaptation and after-effects of double support were surprising to us because previous work showed that halting the adaptation of step position (Sout≈0) limited the correction of spatial errors (defined as SA) (Malone et al., 2012). In an analogous manner, we anticipated that preventing the adaptation of step times (Tout≈0) during split-belt walking was going to limit the adaptation of double support asymmetry (i.e., temporal error Malone et al., 2012). However, we observed that individuals prioritize differently the correction of spatial and temporal asymmetries: they minimize temporal asymmetries, but not spatial ones. This might be because double support time is the transition period when the body mass is transferred from one leg to the other, which is demanding in terms of energy expenditure (Perry, 1992). Therefore, double support symmetry might be critical for efficient body transfer between the limbs (Kuo et al., 2005; Ruina et al., 2005). Taken together our results suggests that the motor system prioritizes the maintenance of double support symmetry, which might be critical for balance control in bipedal locomotion.

4.5. Explicit vs. Implicit Processes in Locomotor Adaptation

Our study contributes to recent efforts to unveil the potential interaction between explicit corrections and implicit sensorimotor recalibration in locomotion (Malone et al., 2012; Long et al., 2016; Roemmich et al., 2016; Statton et al., 2016; Maeda et al., 2017). Interestingly, we found that preventing foot adjustments during split-belt walking significantly reduced post-adaptation effects compared to the control group. This was also observed when using explicit corrections to reduce the adjustment of foot placement in response to a 2:1 speed belt ratio (Malone et al., 2012) but not in response to a larger 3:1 speed belt ratio (Long et al., 2016). Notably, after-effects following the 3:1 perturbation were equally large with or without explicit corrections during the split condition (Long et al., 2016). One interpretation for these results is that the implicit sensorimotor adaptation in walking is scaled with perturbation magnitude. Thus, explicit corrections preventing foot adjustments in the split condition will have a lesser impact on after-effects induced by large perturbations. This interpretation is consistent with the proportional relation between perturbation size and after-effects upon experiencing unexpected constant forces (Green et al., 2010; Torres-Oviedo and Bastian, 2012; Yokoyama et al., 2018), contrasting the fixed amount of implicit sensorimotor recalibration upon visuomotor perturbations (Kim et al., 2018).

4.6. Study Implications

We provide a novel approach for manipulating stance time, which is a major deficit in stroke survivors (Patterson et al., 2008). It would be interesting to determine if this type of feedback overground or on a regular treadmill could lead to gait improvements post-stroke as those induced by split-belt walking (Reisman et al., 2013; Lewek et al., 2018). Our results also indicate that manipulating the adaptation of movements in the temporal domain alters movements in the spatial domain, suggesting that spatial and temporal deficits in individuals with cortical lesions (Malone and Bastian, 2014; Finley et al., 2015) cannot be treated in complete isolation. Only the correction of timing asymmetries through error-based sensorimotor adaptation could occur while preventing the adaptation of spatial ones, as we did in the spatial feedback group. However, the opposite is not possible, at least with the temporal feedback task that we used.

Data Availability

The datasets generated and analyzed for this study can be found in the Figshare repository [https://figshare.com/articles/ExplicitTemporal_SpatialModulations_mat/8145962]. The raw data supporting the conclusions of this manuscript will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation, to any qualified researcher. Requests to access the datasets should be directed to gelsyto@pitt.edu.

Ethics Statement

This study was carried out in accordance with the recommendations of University of Pittsburgh Institutional Review Board (IRB). The protocol was approved by the University of Pittsburgh Institutional Review Board. All subjects gave written informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, except for registration in a database.

Author Contributions

MG-R and NV equally contributed to data acquisition and processing. They also contributed in the interpretation of the data and final approval of the version to be published, and agreement to be accountable for all aspects of the work. GT-O contributions include conception and design of the work, analysis of the data, writing a complete draft of the manuscript, revising work for important intellectual content, final approval of the version to be published, and agreement to be accountable for all aspects of the work.

Funding

The project was funded by National Science Foundation (NSF1535036), and American Heart Association (AHA 15SDG25710041).

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

The authors acknowledge the valuable input from Pablo Iturralde and Carly Sombric.

References

Afzal, M. R., Oh, M. K., Lee, C. H., Park, Y. S., and Yoon, J. (2015). A portable gait asymmetry rehabilitation system for individuals with stroke using a vibrotactile feedback. Biomed. Res. Int. 2015:375638. doi: 10.1155/2015/375638

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Avraham, G., Leib, R., Pressman, A., Simo, L. S., Karniel, A., Shmuelof, L., et al. (2017). State-based delay representation and its transfer from a game of pong to reaching and tracking. eNeuro 4, 1–30. doi: 10.1523/ENEURO.0179-17.2017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Breska, A., and Ivry, R. B. (2018). Double dissociation of single-interval and rhythmic temporal prediction in cerebellar degeneration and Parkinson's disease. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 115, 12283–12288. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1810596115

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Choi, J. T., Vining, E. P., Reisman, D. S., and Bastian, A. J. (2009). Walking flexibility after hemispherectomy: split-belt treadmill adaptation and feedback control. Brain 132, 722–733. doi: 10.1093/brain/awn333

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Darmohray, D. M., Jacobs, J. R., Marques, H. G., and Carey, M. R. (2019). Spatial and temporal locomotor learning in mouse cerebellum. Neuron 102, 217–231. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2019.01.038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Drew, T., and Marigold, D. S. (2015). Taking the next step: cortical contributions to the control of locomotion. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 33, 25–33. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2015.01.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Finley, J. M., Long, A., Bastian, A. J., and Torres-Oviedo, G. (2015). Spatial and temporal control contribute to step length asymmetry during split-belt adaptation and hemiparetic gait. Neurorehabil. Neural Repair 29, 786–795. doi: 10.1177/1545968314567149

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Green, D. A., Bunday, K. L., Bowen, J., Carter, T., and Bronstein, A. M. (2010). What does autonomic arousal tell us about locomotor learning? Neuroscience 170, 42–53. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2010.06.079

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, H. E., Morehead, J. R., Parvin, D. E., Moazzezi, R., and Ivry, R. B. (2018). Invariant errors reveal limitations in motor correction rather than constraints on error sensitivity. Commun. Biol. 1:19. doi: 10.1038/s42003-018-0021-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krishnan, V., Khoo, I., Marayong, P., DeMars, K., and Cormack, J. (2016). Gait training in chronic stroke using walk-even feedback device: a pilot study. Neurosci. J. 2016, 1–8. doi: 10.1155/2016/6808319

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuo, A. D., Donelan, J. M., and Ruina, A. (2005). Energetic consequences of walking like an inverted pendulum: step-to-step transitions energetic consequences of walking like an inverted pendulum: step-to-step transitions. Excerc. Sport Sci. Rev. 33, 88–97. doi: 10.1097/00003677-200504000-00006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lafreniere-Roula, M., and McCrea, D. A. (2005). Deletions of rhythmic motoneuron activity during fictive locomotion and scratch provide clues to the Organization of the Mammalian Central Pattern Generator. J. Neurophysiol. 94, 1120–1132. doi: 10.1152/jn.00216.2005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lewek, M. D., Braun, C. H., Wutzke, C. J., and Giuliani, C. (2018). The role of movement errors in modifying spatiotemporal gait asymmetry post-stroke: a randomized controlled trial. Clin. Rehabil. 32, 161–172. doi: 10.1177/0269215517723056

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Long, A. W., Finley, J. M., and Bastian, A. J. (2015). A marching-walking hybrid induces step length adaptation and transfers to natural walking. J. Neurophysiol. 113, 3905–14. doi: 10.1152/jn.00779.2014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Long, A. W., Roemmich, R. T., and Bastian, A. J. (2016). Blocking trial-by-trial error correction does not interfere with motor learning in human walking. J. Neurophysiol. 115, 2341–2348. doi: 10.1152/jn.00941.2015

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maeda, R. S., O'Connor, S. M., Donelan, J. M., and Marigold, D. S. (2017). Foot placement relies on state estimation during visually guided walking. J. Neurophysiol. 117, 480–491. doi: 10.1152/jn.00015.2016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Malone, L. A., and Bastian, A. J. (2010). Thinking about walking: effects of conscious correction versus distraction on locomotor adaptation. J. Neurophysiol. 103, 1954–1962. doi: 10.1152/jn.00832.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Malone, L. A., and Bastian, A. J. (2014). Spatial and temporal asymmetries in gait predict split-belt adaptation behavior in stroke. Neurorehabil. Neural Repair 28, 230–240. doi: 10.1177/1545968313505912

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Malone, L. A., Bastian, A. J., and Torres-Oviedo, G. (2012). How does the motor system correct for errors in time and space during locomotor adaptation? J. Neurophysiol. 108, 672–683. doi: 10.1152/jn.00391.2011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marigold, D. S., and Drew, T. (2017). Posterior parietal cortex estimates the relationship between object and body location during locomotion. eLife 6, 1–24. doi: 10.7554/eLife.28143

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mariscal, D. M., Iturralde, P. A., and Torres-Oviedo, G. (2018). Augmenting cognitive load during split-belt walking increaes the generalization of motor memories across walking contexts. bioRxiv. doi: 10.1101/470930

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mawase, F., Bar-Haim, S., and Shmuelof, L. (2017). Formation of long-term locomotor memories is associated with functional connectivity changes in the cerebellar thalamic cortical network. J. Neurosci. 37, 349–361. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2733-16.2017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mawase, F., Shmuelof, L., Bar-Haim, S., and Karniel, A. (2014). Savings in locomotor adaptation explained by changes in learning parameters following initial adaptation. J. Neurophysiol. 111, 1444–1454. doi: 10.1152/jn.00734.2013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Patrick, S. K., Musselman, K. E., Tajino, J., Ou, H.-C., Bastian, A. J., and Yang, J. F. (2014). Prior experience but not size of error improves motor learning on the split-belt treadmill in young children. PLoS ONE 9:e93349. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0093349

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Patterson, K. K., Parafianowicz, I., Danells, C. J., Closson, V., Verrier, M. C., Staines, W. R., et al. (2008). Gait asymmetry in community-ambulating stroke survivors. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 89, 304–310. doi: 10.1016/j.apmr.2007.08.142

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perry, J. (1992). “Total limb function,” in Gait Analysis: Normal and Pathological Function, Chapter 9 (Thorofare, NJ: Slack Incorporated), 149–167.

Reisman, D. S., Block, H. J., and Bastian, A. J. (2005). Interlimb coordination during locomotion: what can be adapted and stored? J. Neurophysiol. 94, 2403–2415. doi: 10.1152/jn.00089.2005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reisman, D. S., McLean, H., Keller, J., Danks, K. A., and Bastian, A. J. (2013). Repeated split-belt treadmill training improves poststroke step length asymmetry. Neurorehabil. Neural Repair 27, 460–468. doi: 10.1177/1545968312474118

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reisman, D. S., Wityk, R., Silver, K., and Bastian, A. J. (2007). Locomotor adaptation on a split-belt treadmill can improve walking symmetry post-stroke. Brain 130, 1861–1872. doi: 10.1093/brain/awm035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roemmich, R. T., and Bastian, A. J. (2015). Two ways to save a newly learned motor pattern. J. Neurophysiol. 113, 3519–3530. doi: 10.1152/jn.00965.2014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roemmich, R. T., Long, A. W., and Bastian, A. J. (2016). Seeing the errors you feel enhances locomotor performance but not learning. Curr. Biol. 26, 2707–2716. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2016.08.012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ruina, A., Bertram, J. E. A., and Srinivasan, M. (2005). A collisional model of the energetic cost of support work qualitatively explains leg sequencing in walking and galloping, pseudo-elastic leg behavior in running and the walk-to-run transition. J. Theor. Biol. 237, 170–192. doi: 10.1016/j.jtbi.2005.04.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rybak, I. A., Shevtsova, N. A., Lafreniere-Roula, M., and McCrea, D. A. (2006). Modelling spinal circuitry involved in locomotor pattern generation: insights from deletions during fictive locomotion. J. Physiol. 577, 617–639. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2006.118703

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sombric, C. J., Harker, H. M., Sparto, P. J., and Torres-Oviedo, G. (2017). Explicit action switching interferes with the context-specificity of motor memories in older adults. Front. Aging Neurosci. 9:40. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2017.00040

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Statton, M. A., Toliver, A., and Bastian, A. J. (2016). A dual-learning paradigm can simultaneously train multiple characteristics of walking. J. Neurophysiol. 115, 2692–2700. doi: 10.1152/jn.00090.2016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Statton, M. A., Vazquez, A., Morton, S. M., Vasudevan, E. V., and Bastian, A. J. (2018). Making sense of cerebellar contributions to perceptual and motor adaptation. Cerebellum 17, 111–121. doi: 10.1007/s12311-017-0879-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Torres-Oviedo, G., and Bastian, A. J. (2010). Seeing is believing: effects of visual contextual cues on learning and transfer of locomotor adaptation. J. Neurosci. 30, 17015–17022. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4205-10.2010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Torres-Oviedo, G., and Bastian, A. J. (2012). Natural error patterns enable transfer of motor learning to novel contexts. J. Neurophysiol. 107, 346–356. doi: 10.1152/jn.00570.2011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vasudevan, E. V. L., Torres-Oviedo, G., Morton, S. M., Yang, J. F., and Bastian, J. A. (2011). Younger is not always better: development of locomotor adaptation from childhood to adulthood. J. Neurosci. 31, 3055–3065. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5781-10.2011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vervoort, D., den Otter, A. R., Buurke, T. J. W., Vuillerme, N., Hortobágyi, T., and Lamoth, C. J. C. (2019). Effects of aging and task prioritization on split-belt gait adaptation. Front. Aging Neurosci. 11:10. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2019.00010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yokoyama, H., Sato, K., Ogawa, T., Yamamoto, S. I., Nakazawa, K., and Kawashima, N. (2018). Characteristics of the gait adaptation process due to split-belt treadmill walking under a wide range of right-left speed ratios in humans. PLoS ONE 13:e0194875. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0194875

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: locomotion, motor learning, split-belt, spatio-temporal, sensorimotor adaptation, kinematics

Citation: Gonzalez-Rubio M, Velasquez NF and Torres-Oviedo G (2019) Explicit Control of Step Timing During Split-Belt Walking Reveals Interdependent Recalibration of Movements in Space and Time. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 13:207. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2019.00207

Received: 14 March 2019; Accepted: 03 June 2019;
Published: 03 July 2019.

Edited by:

David W. Franklin, Technical University of Munich, Germany

Reviewed by:

Ryan Thomas Roemmich, Kennedy Krieger Institute, United States
Sjoerd M. Bruijn, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, Netherlands
Soichiro Fujiki, Dokkyo Medical University, Japan

Copyright © 2019 Gonzalez-Rubio, Velasquez and Torres-Oviedo. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Gelsy Torres-Oviedo, gelsyto@pitt.edu

These authors have contributed equally to this work

Download