Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Investigating the Associations of Self-Rated Health: Heart Rate Variability Is More Strongly Associated than Inflammatory and Other Frequently Used Biomarkers in a Cross Sectional Occupational Sample

  • Marc N. Jarczok,

    Affiliation Mannheim Institute of Public Health, Social and Preventive Medicine, Mannheim Medical Faculty, Heidelberg University, Mannheim, Germany

  • Marcus E. Kleber,

    Affiliation Mannheim Institute of Public Health, Social and Preventive Medicine, Mannheim Medical Faculty, Heidelberg University, Mannheim, Germany

  • Julian Koenig ,

    Koenig.393@osu.edu

    Affiliation The Ohio State University, Department of Psychology, Columbus, Ohio, United States of America

  • Adrian Loerbroks,

    Affiliation Institute of Occupational and Social Medicine, Medical Faculty, Heinrich-Heine-University Düsseldorf, Düsseldorf, Germany

  • Raphael M. Herr,

    Affiliations Department of Clinical Psychology, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands, Mannheim Institute of Public Health, Social and Preventive Medicine, Mannheim Medical Faculty, Heidelberg University, Mannheim, Germany

  • Kristina Hoffmann,

    Affiliation Mannheim Institute of Public Health, Social and Preventive Medicine, Mannheim Medical Faculty, Heidelberg University, Mannheim, Germany

  • Joachim E. Fischer,

    Affiliation Mannheim Institute of Public Health, Social and Preventive Medicine, Mannheim Medical Faculty, Heidelberg University, Mannheim, Germany

  • Yael Benyamini,

    Affiliation Bob Shapell School of Social Work, Tel Aviv University, Tel Aviv, Israel

  • Julian F. Thayer

    Affiliations The Ohio State University, Department of Psychology, Columbus, Ohio, United States of America, Mannheim Institute of Public Health, Social and Preventive Medicine, Mannheim Medical Faculty, Heidelberg University, Mannheim, Germany

Abstract

The present study aimed to investigate the possible mechanisms linking a single–item measure of global self-rated health (SRH) with morbidity by comparing the association strengths between SRH with markers of autonomic nervous system (ANS) function, inflammation, blood glucose and blood lipids. Cross–sectional comprehensive health–check data of 3947 working adults (age 42±11) was used to calculate logistic regressions, partial correlations and compare correlation strength using Olkins Z. Adjusted logistic regression models showed a negative association between SRH (higher values indicating worse health) and measures of heart rate variability (HRV). Glycemic markers were positively associated with poor SRH. No adjusted association was found with inflammatory markers, BP or lipids. In both unadjusted and adjusted linear models Pearson’s correlation strength was significantly higher between SRH with HRV measures compared to SRH with other biomarkers. This is the first study investigating the association of ANS function and SRH. We showed that a global measure of SRH is associated with HRV, and that all measures of ANS function were significantly more strongly associated with SRH than any other biomarker. The current study supports the hypothesis that the extent of brain–body communication, as indexed by HRV, is associated with self-rated health.

Introduction

Self-rated health (SRH), a simple question asking individuals to rate their health in general, has consistently been found to predict mortality, morbidity, and other health outcomes [1]. This is not surprising in itself, as SRH is associated with many indicators of physical and mental health (see meta-analysis by Pinquart [2]), such as functional ability, depression, and chronic pain, as well as with social risk factors such as socioeconomic status (e.g. income disparities), demographics (age and gender), work stress and leadership [3]. The more intriguing finding is that in most studies that have examined this, SRH remained a significant predictor of future health outcomes even after controlling for many relevant covariates that include known risk factors for poor health [1].

The single item SRH measure has demonstrated good reproducibility, reliability, and strong concurrent and discriminant scale performance with an established more detailed subjective health status measure [4]. Its validity has been consistently supported by hundreds of studies reporting its associations with physician ratings and other health measures and its prediction of future mortality and morbidity. Further support for its role as an integrative evaluation of one’s health and not a momentary assessment comes from studies showing that it is not sensitive to mood inductions [5] or recent acute illnesses [6], and, while it has been found to be associated with measures of personality, its associations with objective medical burden remain even after controlling for personality [7]. Furthermore, a 22-year follow up of the General Social Survey showed an increasing predictive validity of self-rated health on of mortality [8].

One of the explanations proposed for this independent association of SRH with mortality and morbidity is that SRH is more inclusive than the covariates typically used in studies and as a single item, provides an optimal integration of many types of information, including external (e.g. diagnoses) and internal information (e.g. bodily sensations) [9,10]. Thus, SRH may reflect diseases at pre-clinical stages as well as more accurately reflect the cumulative effects of comorbidity [9,11]. This explanation is supported by findings showing that SRH is strongly associated with tiredness [12], declines in timed gait [13] and walking speed [14], and that vigor interacts with functional capacity to predict changes in SRH [15]. Moreover, the "valid core" of SRH has been found to include feelings of fatigue, lack of energy, and diminished activity [6].

In line with these self-reports, SRH has been found to be associated with a wide range of biomarkers [16,17], including inflammatory markers [18], even after controlling for a variety of health and psychosocial measures [19]. The association of SRH with mortality is similar to [20] or even superior to that of a panel of biomarkers [21,22]. In sum, while the association of SRH with future health diminishes with increasing adjustments, it is typically not eliminated, suggesting that SRH is a predictor of mortality because SRH reflects the state of the human organism and is likely to be based at least in part on interoceptive processes [11].

A possible but as yet uninvestigated mechanism that could link the various sources of information that feed into SRH is autonomic nervous system (ANS) activity. One widely used measure of ANS activity is the beat-to-beat variations in the cardiac rhythm as indexed by heart rate variability (HRV). HRV may be more than just an index of healthy heart function and may in fact serve as an easily measured output of the brain’s integrative system for adaptive regulation [23]. Like SRH, HRV has been independently associated with morbidity and mortality from a wide range of disorders including metabolic syndrome (MetS) [24], low-grade inflammation [25,26], fatigue [27], work stress [28], and cardiovascular disease [29]. Thus, HRV, like SRH, may be a more inclusive index of health and may be a more integrative measure of external and internal health-related information. In sum, HRV seems to be a promising physiological correlate of SRH yet surprisingly, based on a systematic literature search in Medline (via PubMed) and PsychInfo (1st April 2014), the relationship between SRH and HRV has not been investigated.

Therefore, the following analysis aimed to investigate (1) the associations between a single–item measure of global SRH and HRV in a large sample of working adults; and (2) the relative strength of associations between global SRH and HRV versus other frequently used biomarkers including inflammatory markers. It is hypothesized that SRH would be associated with ANS activity as indexed by HRV and that this association would be more pronounced in comparison to other frequently used but less inclusive biomarkers.

Methods

Study Population and Design

A total of 9730 workers employed in four distinct plants were invited between September 2009 and May 2011 to a voluntary comprehensive health–check comprising a questionnaire and a medical examination in Southern Germany. A total of 4881 employees (mean age 41 SD 11 from 18–65 years, 22% females) accepted the invitation to the health-check (response rate 50.2%). Complete information on all used measurements was necessary to rule out composition effects when comparing coefficients between measurements resulting in an analysis sample of N = 3947 (80.9%). All participants were Caucasian and assessed by an agent independent from the employer who conducted the health risk assessments and data collection (HealthVision Ltd., Berlingen, Switzerland). The Ethical Committee of the Mannheim Medical Faculty, Heidelberg University approved this secondary data analysis (2010-296E-MA). All participants gave written informed consent prior to examination. For more details on measurements and population please see [24,30].

Measures

SRH was assessed using a single item asking about overall health status based on the past four weeks: “In general, would you say your health is: Excellent (1), Very Good (2), Good (3), Fair (4), Poor (5)”.

Biomarkers

Raw beat–to–beat intervals (IBI) from 24h HR-recordings were analyzed by researchers at the Centre for Neuropsychological Research (University of Trier, Germany) according to the Task Force Guidelines [31]. The 24-hour IBI-data were decomposed into 5.35 minutes blocks. The “ANS-Explorer” Software [32] was used to calculate time domain and frequency domain (Fast Fourier Transform-based) parameters as well as artifact rate per block. The mean IBI, root mean square of successive differences (RMSSD), standard deviation of all N–N (normal–to–normal) intervals (SDNN), frequency domain power in the low (LF) (0.04–0.15 Hz) and high frequency (HF) (0.15–0.4 Hz) were categorized into tertiles (Table 1). RMSSD and HF are known to reflect primarily parasympathetic mediated HRV while SDNN and LF are known to reflect both, sympathetic and parasympathetic mediated HRV.

thumbnail
Table 1. Classification of biomarkers into (clinical) categories.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0117196.t001

Demographic, medical and lifestyle variables were obtained from an online questionnaire. The questionnaire had to be completed prior to being able to schedule the medical examination. All participants were enrolled and examined between 10 a.m. and 5 p.m. on a typical workday (Monday to Friday) during work hours. Upon arrival a medical examination was performed and the HR-recorder was attached. The next morning, between 7 a.m. and 9 a.m., a fasting blood sample was collected from all individuals. Samples were transported to a commercial laboratory (Synlab, Augsburg, Germany) within 2 hours of sample collection and analyzed within 24-hours.

Inflammatory markers (C-reactive protein [CRP], white blood count [WBC]), blood glucose (Fasting plasma glucose [FPG], glycosylated hemoglobin [HbA1c]), and blood lipids (Total cholesterol [CHOL], triglyceride [TRI], low density lipoprotein [LDL], high density lipoprotein [HDL]) were determined using routine laboratory analyzers (for details see [24]) Blood pressure (BP) using the oscillometric technique was recorded twice using the CRITIKON Dinamap Portable Adult/Pediatric and Neonatal Vital Signs Monitor (Model 8100). Measurements were taken from the dominant arm in the seated position after a 5-minute rest period. A study physician repeated the reading using sphygmomanometry if one or more BP values exceeded 135mmHg (systolic) or 90mmHg (diastolic). The arithmetic mean of all two to three measurements was calculated.

Other covariates

Work stress was assessed by a validated short version of the Effort–Reward Imbalance (ERI) questionnaire [33]. According to a predefined algorithm, a ratio between the “effort” and “reward” scales was calculated to quantify the degree of mismatch between high “cost” and low “gain” at the individual level (weighted by number of items). Here, values >1 indicate failed reciprocity between efforts spent at work and rewards received in turn.

Subjective sleep quality was assessed by a continuous score based on a translation of the 4–item Jenkins sleep quality scales with higher values indicating worse sleep quality [34]. Socio-economic measures were span of control (0, 1–4, 5–20, >20) and hierarchical position (division manager, team manager, foreman, skilled worker, unskilled worker). Behavioral factors included A) smoking status (non-smokers, ex-smokers, or current smokers), B) alcohol drinking during the past 6 months by four questions on the frequency of wine, beer, spirits or cocktails (6 to 7 days a week; 3 to 5 days a week; Once or twice a week; Once or twice a month; Never) and C) frequency of vigorous physical activity during the past 6 months by one question (three times or more often per week; More than once a week; About once a week; One to three times per month; Hardly ever).

The components of the MetS were defined according to the joint interim statement to harmonize the MetS [35]. We considered MetS to be present if at least three of the following criteria were met: triglycerides (TRI) ≥1.7 mmol/l (150 mg/dl) or use of medication for hypertriglyceridemia was reported; high-density lipoprotein (HDL) ≤1.0 mmol/l (40 mg/dl) for males and ≤1.3 mmol/l (50 mg/dl) for females, waist circumference ≥94 cm for males or ≥80 cm for females, systolic BP ≥130 mmHg and/or diastolic BP ≥85mmHg or use of an anti-hypertensive medication was reported, fasting plasma glucose (FPG) ≥5.6mmol/l (100mg/dl) or use of a hypoglycemic medication was reported.

Statistical Analysis

The distribution of all continuous variables was determined and transformations were used as necessary to meet the assumptions of modeling. Differences between the full sample and the analysis sample were determined by t-test for continuous and χ2 test for categorical variables. A four-stage approach was applied:

First, bivariate associations between SRH and continuous biomarkers were assessed using Bonferroni-corrected Pearson’s correlation as well as nonparametric Kendall’s rank correlation coefficient τb. Compared to Pearson’s correlation, Kendall τb does neither assume a normal distribution nor equidistant scaling of the correlated variables. In addition, Kendall τb makes adjustments for ties. Values of τb range from −1 (100% negative association) to +1 (100% positive association). A value of zero indicates the absence of association. This could be important, since the various biomarkers are usually neither normally distributed nor equidistant to a five-point Likert scale (SRH). On the other hand, Pearson correlations are easier to compare.

Second, multiply–adjusted partial correlation coefficients and partial rank correlation coefficients Kendall τb were used to reflect the association between SRH and a biomarker independent of covariates (demographics, lifestyle, sleep quality, and work stress) in the model.

Third, we used one sample Olkin’s Z to systematically compare the correlation strength (gained from step one and two) between the associations of SRH and each biomarker additionally adjusting for multiple comparisons by Bonferroni-correction. The null hypothesis of Olkins Z is that the correlation strength between SRH and biomarker A is equal to the correlation strength of SRH and biomarker B.

Fourth, biomarkers were categorized into either clinically relevant categories or into quantiles if no clinical cut offs exist (Table 1) to show the relative strength of association (both unadjusted and adjusted) in odds ratios by each category for all biomarkers, which are then summarized in a forest plot. Here, the point estimate and it’s respective confidence interval can be easily compared across the logistic regressions per biomarker. SRH was dichotomized with individuals indicated fair (12.79%) or poor (0.91%) SRH versus those indicating good (49.91%), very good (31.42%) or excellent (4.97%) SRH. Hierarchical multiple logistic regression models with dichotomized SRH as the dependent and each categorized biomarker as the independent variable adjusting block wise for demographics, lifestyle, sleep quality, and work stress were calculated. Likelihood–ratio tests were applied after each block to test logistic model improvement. The overall contribution of variables to the model was tested using Wald–tests. Covariates that had no association with SRH and did not contribute significantly to the overall model fit in logistic regressions were excluded from final analysis.

Imputing missing SRH values (2.01%) by missing value regression had no impact on any results and are reported here. A total of 3947 individual data sets with complete data regarding questionnaire, medical data, and HRV assessment remained. STATA 12.1MP (College Station, TX) was used for data management and analysis.

Results

The study sample consisted primarily of male workers (78%). The majority of participants were skilled workers (64%) as well as project or division managers (29%) without (78%) and with (22%) leadership responsibilities. Healthy behavior seems to be prominent with 81% non-smoker, 62% drinking alcohol maximal twice a week, 70% doing sweat–rich activities at least once a week, and having less than three components of the MetS (77%). We found some differences between the full sample and the analysis sample regarding blood lipids and glucose levels but the magnitudes of the differences are below clinically relevant levels (e.g. FPG-difference = 2.08mg/dl) (Table 2).

thumbnail
Table 2. Differences between analysis sample and missing sample.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0117196.t002

Bivariate associations

Bonferroni–corrected correlations (both Pearson’s r and Kendall τb) indicate the highest significant associations between our indices of ANS function and SRH. Higher SRH values (= worse health) were significantly and negatively associated with vagally–mediated HRV (RMSSD r = −0.22; HF r = −0.21) as well as with mixed sympathetic and parasympathetic measures (SDNN r = −0.22; LF r = −0.20). Additionally, rank order Kendall τb correlations reveal a comparable order of association strength (Table 3).

thumbnail
Table 3. Pearson’s correlation and Kendall τb of self-rated health (SRH) with ANS function measure and other biomarkers.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0117196.t003

Fig. 1 shows the mean RMSSD per SRH category and indicates a linear trend. Other biomarkers and SRH were also significantly (Bonferroni corrected) correlated but with smaller effect sizes such as blood glucose levels (FPG r = 0.13 and HbA1c r = 0.13) or inflammatory markers (CRP r = 0.13 and WBC r = 0.07). It is important to note that correlations of SRH and ANS function measurements were significantly stronger (compared using Olkins Z) during nighttime recordings (RMSSD r = −0.21; HF r = −0.21) compared to daytime recordings (RMSSD r = −0.19; HF r = −0.19) of HRV (data not shown). Every correlation of SRH with ANS function measures was significantly larger when compared with every correlation between SRH and other biomarkers using Olkins Z (Table 4).

thumbnail
Fig 1. Mean RMSSD and self-rated health (error bars 95% CI).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0117196.g001

thumbnail
Table 4. Correlation strength comparison (Olkins Z) of self-rated health (SRH) with ANS measure or other biomarker.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0117196.t004

Fig. 2 presents odds ratios and their respective 95% confidence intervals from bivariate (unadjusted) logistic regression models with poor SRH as the dependent and each biomarker as the independent variable. Clinical cut offs or highest tertiles (representing worse health) are consistently associated with higher odds ratios for poor SRH. In these bivariate analyses, HRV measures seem to have the strongest associations with SRH independent of the measure of association used.

thumbnail
Fig 2. Forest plot showing unadjusted Odds Ratios (OR) of different biomarkers predicting poor self rated health: # Females / Males.

$ Age <20 / 20–<30 / 30–<40 / >40. Reading example: All else being equal the odds of indicating poor SRH for participants with HbA1c being larger 6.5% compared to participants with HbA1c below 6% is increased by the factor 6.14.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0117196.g002

SRH is significantly correlated (Bonferroni–corrected bivariate Pearson’s r and Kendall τb) with sleep quality, work stress, age, and physical activity. Also work stress is significantly and positively correlated with all measures of blood glucose, blood lipids (not HDL), diastolic BP, as well as negatively correlated with all measures of ANS function. All correlations had a small effect size (Table 5). Socioeconomic status measures, gender, and smoking status were not associated with poor SRH (not bivariate nor in multivariate models) and thus were excluded from subsequent models.

thumbnail
Table 5. Bonferroni corrected Pearson’s correlation and Kendall τb for potential confounders with self-rated health (SRH).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0117196.t005

Multivariate associations

The forest plot (Fig. 3) presents the same models but adjusted for age, lifestyle, and work stress. Adjusted logistic regression models showed a negative association with measures of HRV comparing the third (worse health) vs. first (better health) tertile of vagally mediated HRV (RMSSD OR = 1.64 95%CI [1.23–2.19] p = 0.001; HF OR = 1.38 [1.03–1.85] p = 0.034) and both sympathetic and parasympathetic mediated HRV (SDNN OR = 1.46 [1.12–1.91] p = 0.006; LF OR = 1.39 [1.05–1.84] p = 0.021). Glycemic markers (HbA1c >6.5% OR = 2.90 [1.65–5.09] p<0.001; FPG >126mg/dl OR = 3.49 [1.72–7.06] p = 0.001) were positively associated with poor SRH. No adjusted association was found with any inflammatory markers, BP, and blood lipid measures (Fig. 3). Work stress and sleep quality were significantly, and independent of HRV, positively associated with poor SRH in all models (ERI OR 2.3 [1.89–2.80] p <0.001; Jenkins OR 1.2 [1.18–1.24] p<0.001). This means, the higher the self-reported work-stress the higher the odds ratio for worse SRH. The OR of the HRV variables were slightly attenuated after entering work stress and sleep quality into the models but remained significant and of a relevant effect size. Full models had a pseudo–R2 between 17–19%.

thumbnail
Fig 3. Forest plot showing adjusted Odds Ratios (OR) of different biomarkers predicting poor self rated health; # Females / Males.

$ Age <20 / 20–<30 / 30–<40 / >40. Adjusted for age, sport, alcohol, sleep quality, and work stress. Reading example: All else being equal the odds of indicating poor SRH for participants with HbA1c being larger 6.5% compared to participants with HbA1c below 6% is increased by the factor 3.13.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0117196.g003

Discussion

Principal findings

This is the first study to show that a global measure of SRH was associated with ANS function as indexed by HRV. The strength of associations of traditional biomarkers with ANS function measures were compared and it was shown that all measures of ANS function were significantly more strongly associated with SRH than any of the other assessed biomarkers in a healthy working population. These findings provide valuable information about the mechanisms by which SRH is related to mortality and morbidity.

Comparison with existing studies

Consistent with prior research, the present study found that biomarkers associated with inflammation (CRP and WBC) and the MetS (FPG and HbA1c) had significant bivariate associations with SRH. The magnitudes of the associations in the present study are similar to those previously reported [17,22,24,36] but somewhat smaller than reported by others [18]. For example, our unadjusted association between SRH and CRP of r = −0.11 is similar to the −0.16 association found by Janszky [36] but smaller than the r = −0.28 association found by Christian [18]. The reasons for this difference are unclear. Both the Janszky [36] and Christian [18] samples were older than the present sample and one [36] sample was only women whereas our sample was significantly larger and primarily men (Total N = 3357 versus N = 232 for [36] and N = 250 for [18]. However our results are consistent with the majority of the extant literature whereas the Christian [18] findings are somewhat larger. Importantly in the adjusted models, the differences between our findings (r = −0.07) and the Christian [18] study (r = −0.12) narrowed and remained consistent with the Janszky [36] study (r = −0.12). Furthermore, the reported associations between HRV measures and the other biomarkers are also comparable to the existing literature e.g. [2426,29,37,38]. Thus the present findings with respect to the association between inflammatory and MetS biomarkers, and SRH are comparable to others and support the generalizability of the present findings.

Consistent with our hypothesis HRV is a more inclusive and integrative index than other frequently investigated biomarkers. There are several compelling reasons why this might be the case. First, it has been shown consistently that measures of vagally-mediated HRV have strong independent associations with mortality and morbidity [29,39] and so does SRH [1]. Second, measures of vagally-mediated HRV are independently associated with a range of biomarkers including inflammatory and MetS biomarkers [2426]. Third, vagally-mediated HRV is associated with emotional regulation and dysregulation [40,41]. The latter two points show that vagally-mediated HRV is associated with both biological measures as well as psychological measures and thus might be more inclusive just as SRH is.

Possible explanations and implication

The vagus nerve innervates a wide range of organ systems in the body and as such is well positioned to provide feedback about the state of the organism to the brain. In fact 80% of the vagal fibers are afferent and thus the vagus has been shown to be important in transmitting diverse types of information to the brain including information concerning immune status, blood glucose levels, and pain [4244]. It is now widely accepted that the ANS is also important in immune function [45,46]. For example, the vagus nerve is known to relay information about peripheral immune status to the brain via interleukin-1 cytokine receptors conveyed by paraganglia cells situated in parasympathetic ganglia [47]. In addition, efferent vagal activity via the release of acetylcholine inhibits release of pro-inflammatory cytokines and has been termed the cholinergic anti-inflammatory pathway [48].

It is less well known that the vagus is also involved in glucose regulation. Vagal afferents in the hepatic portal contain glucagon-like peptide-1 receptors that convey information about peripheral glucose status to the brain. In addition, vagal efferent fibers innervate the liver and the kidneys and play a role in glucose regulation [4951]. The association between vagal activity and glucose levels has been previously shown to be stronger during nighttime [24]. Overall, the action of the vagus nerve is a common denominator for both immune and glucose regulation. Moreover the vagus nerve is also important in pain regulation [44,5254].

Thus measures of HRV may capture a broader range of function across various bodily systems than other biomarkers. Taken together the literature suggests an important role for the ANS, particularly the vagus nerve, in the transmission of important information about bodily functions to the brain. Importantly, efferent vagal activity also seems to be significant in regulating these same bodily functions. This role is also supported by our result of a significantly stronger correlation between SRH and vagally mediated HRV during nighttime (as opposed to daytime values) a period of rest where the parasympathetic nervous system and hence the vagus is expected to be dominant. Better sleep quality is associated with better SRH [55] and insufficient sleep is associated with poor SRH [56,57]. Likewise, better sleep quality is associated with higher measures of heart rate variability [58]. Additional analysis from logistic regression models on poor SRH adjusted for age, sex, sport, and alcohol reveal higher OR for poor SRH with lower tertiles of HRV in the nighttime models (compared to daytime models). Coefficients are attenuated after additional adjustment for sleep quality, but the patterns and statistical significances remain. This leads to the conclusion that sleep quality accounts for some but not all differences between day and nighttime measures of HRV predicting SRH. Thus, HRV may serve as an important index of this bi-directional communication across diverse bodily systems.

The exact neural concomitants of these bodily functions have also been a topic of investigation. A network of neural structures has been identified that is important in the integration and regulation of many systems of the body and has been termed the medial prefrontal-brainstem axis [59]. An important component of this network is the prefrontal cortex (PFC) that has been linked with immune and glucose regulation. For example, Page et al. [60] have reported that higher circulating glucose levels were associated with greater activity in the medial PFC. Activity in this area increased in response to glucose infusion and was related to decreased interest in food stimuli. Importantly, this inhibitory control over food motivation was absent in obese individuals. In addition, it was recently shown that activity in a network including the medial PFC was reduced in response to glucose ingestion [61]. These findings suggest that the PFC is an important site for the regulation of glucose and eating behavior such that greater activity in this region is associated with euglycemia and context appropriate eating behavior. Of particular relevance to the present study, we have reported that HRV is directly related to activity in the medial PFC [23].

Model of neurovisceral integration

A possible explanation for the stronger negative association of HRV and SRH can be drawn from the neurovisceral integration model of cardiac vagal control. This model integrates autonomic, attentional, and affective systems into a functional and structural network [62]. One of the major structures of this network, the ventromedial PFC is particularly involved in building the meaning of a situation, especially when conceptual information drives affective, physiological, and behavioral responses [63]. Furthermore, frontal cortex structures are particularly involved in visceral sensory processing with an emphasis on the medial prefrontal region [64]. Although SRH is likely to be associated with a distributed brain system rather than a single brain center, the ventromedial PFC could be the nexus between SRH and HRV where the complete information is integrated and processed either consciously or more likely non-consciously.

Additionally, the bi-directional communication between the peripheral nervous system and the brain has been clearly enunciated in the neurovisceral integration model [6568]. The current study therefore supports the hypothesis that the extent of central –peripheral neural feedback and central autonomic network– ANS integration, as indexed by HRV [62,69] is associated with SRH.

Four possible factors have been proposed to explain the validity of SRH as a predictor of health outcomes [9]. Importantly, HRV appears to share many of these factors. First, both SRH and HRV are more inclusive than other biomarkers or disease reports and may be based at least in part on bodily sensations that are accessible only to the individual [11]. Second, they may reflect changes over time in health status [70]. Third, both SRH and HRV may influence (health) behavior. This might be a function of the ability to read, interpret, and react to information from within the body (interoception), the outer world (exteroception) and its specific context, and therefore may guide health-related behavior such as eating. Fourth, both SRH and HRV reflect resources, which enable persons to cope with health threats.

Strengths and limitations of this study

This study adds to the ample evidence of the validity of a single SRH item as a measure of one’s health. However, a single item may have also its limits, which should be further explored. For example, research has identified certain subpopulations such as women or Blacks where the SRH–mortality relationship is less pronounced. Mastery and a more active lifestyle in a variety of domains such as cognitive, physical and social domains has been proposed as a potential explanation in this regards [71]. There are also some other limitations to this study: The relatively weak associations of biomarkers with SRH might be due to low numbers of observations in the categories “fair” and “poor health”, which is not an unusual observation in an occupational cohort due to a potential “healthy worker bias”. However percentages are comparable to others found in the literature e.g. [72,73]. Due to the cross-sectional design of our study causal directions or temporal associations between SRH and biomarkers cannot be concluded. However the predictive value of SRH on mortality or morbidity or the sensitivity to change have been reported elsewhere [70].

This study has several strengths. This is a large occupational sample from four distinct geographical regions within Germany comprised of employees with differing socioeconomic backgrounds rather than being limited to one particular professional group. Furthermore, all participants were from a similar cultural background, so genetic and cultural heterogeneity that might affect SRH are likely minimized. Finally this study accounts for many potential covariates in the statistical analyses and HRV was recorded over 24h.

Conclusion

In sum we report for the first time that poor SRH was inversely associated with vagally-mediated HRV in a large, young healthy working cohort. It has been shown that HRV was more strongly associated with SRH than any other assessed biomarker. The present study provides important insights into the mechanisms by which SRH is able to provide information not readily available from other sources about the health status of individuals. The common ability of both SRH and HRV to provide an integrative index of exteroceptive and interoceptive information relevant to health related perceptions and behaviors offers new insights into SRH and HRV. Future research is needed to investigate the temporal and causal association of ANS function and self-rated health.

Acknowledgments

We are indebted to HealthVision Ltd., Berlingen Switzerland for providing the data as well as the Centre for Neuropsychological Research (University of Trier) for providing the HRV computations of the raw data. We gratefully acknowledge Marion Jarczok from the University of Amsterdam for her valuable comments and fruitful discussions during preparation of this manuscript. We thank also Christina Bock from the Mannheim Institute of Public Health for consultation on Olkins Z statistics.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: MNJ MEK AL RMH KH JEF. Performed the experiments: MNJ MEK AL RMH KH. Analyzed the data: MNJ JK YB JFT. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: MNJ JEF JFT. Wrote the paper: MNJ MEK AL RMH KH JEF JK YB JFT.

References

  1. 1. Benyamini Y, Idler EL (1999) Community Studies Reporting Association between Self-Rated Health and Mortality: Additional Studies, 1995 to 1998. Res Aging 21: 392–401. .
  2. 2. Pinquart M (2001) Correlates of subjective health in older adults: A meta-analysis. Psychol Aging 16: 414–426. pmid:11554520
  3. 3. Schmidt B, Loerbroks A, Herr RM, Wilson MG, Jarczok MN, et al. (2014) Associations between supportive leadership and employees self-rated health in an occupational sample. Int J Behav Med 21: 750–756. pmid:24072350
  4. 4. DeSalvo KB, Fisher WP, Tran K, Bloser N, Merrill W, et al. (2006) Assessing measurement properties of two single-item general health measures. Qual Life Res 15: 191–201. pmid:16468076
  5. 5. Barger SD, Burke SM, Limbert MJ (2007) Do induced moods really influence health perceptions? Heal Psychol 26: 85–95. 10.1037/0278-6133.26.1.85.
  6. 6. Benyamini Y, Leventhal EA, Leventhal H (1999) Self-Assessments of Health: What Do People Know that Predicts their Mortality? Res Aging 21: 477–500. .
  7. 7. Chapman BP, Duberstein PR, Sörensen S, Lyness JM (2006) Personality and Perceived Health in Older Adults: The Five Factor Model in Primary Care. J Gerontol Psychol Sci 61B: P362–P365. pmid:17114306
  8. 8. Schnittker J, Bacak V (2014) The increasing predictive validity of self-rated health. PLoS One 9: e84933. pmid:24465452
  9. 9. Benyamini Y (2011) Why does self-rated health predict mortality? An update on current knowledge and a research agenda for psychologists. Psychol Health 26: 1407–1413. pmid:22111660
  10. 10. Idler EL, Benyamini Y (1997) Self-rated health and mortality: a review of twenty-seven community studies. J Health Soc Behav 38: 21–37. pmid:9097506
  11. 11. Jylhä M (2009) What is self-rated health and why does it predict mortality? Towards a unified conceptual model. Soc Sci Med 69: 307–316. pmid:19520474
  12. 12. Singh-Manoux A, Martikainen P, Ferrie J, Zins M, Marmot M, et al. (2006) What does self rated health measure? Results from the British Whitehall II and French Gazel cohort studies. J Epidemiol Community Health 60: 364–372. pmid:16537356
  13. 13. Martinez DJ, Kasl S V, Gill TM, Barry LC (2010) Longitudinal association between self-rated health and timed gait among older persons. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 65: 715–719. pmid:20028951
  14. 14. Ashburner JM, Cauley J a, Cawthon P, Ensrud KE, Hochberg MC, et al. (2011) Self-ratings of health and change in walking speed over 2 years: results from the caregiver-study of osteoporotic fractures. Am J Epidemiol 173: 882–889. pmid:21354990
  15. 15. Shirom A, Toker S, Berliner S, Shapira I, Melamed S (2008) The effects of physical fitness and feeling vigorous on self-rated health. Heal Psychol Off J Div Heal Psychol Am Psychol Assoc 27: 567–575. .
  16. 16. Haseli-Mashhadi N, Pan A, Ye X, Wang J, Qi Q, et al. (2009) Self-Rated Health in middle-aged and elderly Chinese: distribution, determinants and associations with cardio-metabolic risk factors. BMC Public Health 9: 368. pmid:19788754
  17. 17. Yamada C, Moriyama K, Takahashi E (2012) Self-rated health as a comprehensive indicator of lifestyle-related health status. Environ Health Prev Med 17: 457–462. pmid:22426707
  18. 18. Christian LM, Glaser R, Porter K, Malarkey WB, Beversdorf D, et al. (2011) Poorer self-rated health is associated with elevated inflammatory markers among older adults. Psychoneuroendocrinology 36: 1495–1504. pmid:21601365
  19. 19. Goldman N, Glei DA, Chang M-C (2004) The role of clinical risk factors in understanding self-rated health. Ann Epidemiol 14: 49–57. pmid:14664780
  20. 20. Lima-Costa MF, Cesar CC, Chor D, Proietti F a (2012) Self-rated health compared with objectively measured health status as a tool for mortality risk screening in older adults: 10-year follow-up of the Bambuí Cohort Study of Aging. Am J Epidemiol 175: 228–235. pmid:22193172
  21. 21. Haring R, Feng Y, Moock J, Völzke H, Dörr M, et al. (2011) Self-perceived quality of life predicts mortality risk better than a multi-biomarker panel, but the combination of both does best. BMC Med Res Methodol 11: 103. pmid:21749697
  22. 22. Jylhä M, Volpato S, Guralnik JM (2006) Self-rated health showed a graded association with frequently used biomarkers in a large population sample. J Clin Epidemiol 59: 465–471. pmid:16632134
  23. 23. Thayer JF, Ahs F, Fredrikson M, Sollers JJ, Wager TD (2012) A meta-analysis of heart rate variability and neuroimaging studies: implications for heart rate variability as a marker of stress and health. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 36: 747–756. pmid:22178086
  24. 24. Jarczok MN, Li J, Mauss D, Fischer JE, Thayer JF (2013) Heart rate variability is associated with glycemic status after controlling for components of the metabolic syndrome. Int J Cardiol 167: 855–861. pmid:22386703
  25. 25. Thayer JF, Fischer JE (2009) Heart rate variability, overnight urinary norepinephrine and C-reactive protein: evidence for the cholinergic anti-inflammatory pathway in healthy human adults. J Intern Med 265: 439–447. pmid:19019182
  26. 26. Jarczok MN, Koenig J, Mauss D, Fischer JE, Thayer JF (2014) Lower heart rate variability predicts increased level of C-reactive protein 4 years later in healthy, nonsmoking adults. J Intern Med 37: 1810–1823. .
  27. 27. Fagundes CP, Murray DM, Hwang BS, Gouin J-P, Thayer JF, et al. (2011) Sympathetic and parasympathetic activity in cancer-related fatigue: more evidence for a physiological substrate in cancer survivors. Psychoneuroendocrinology 36: 1137–1147. pmid:21388744
  28. 28. Loerbroks A, Schilling O, Haxsen V, Jarczok MN, Thayer JF, et al. (2010) The fruits of ones labor: Effort-reward imbalance but not job strain is related to heart rate variability across the day in 35–44-year-old workers. J Psychosom Res 69: 151–159. pmid:20624513
  29. 29. Thayer JF, Yamamoto SS, Brosschot JF (2010) The relationship of autonomic imbalance, heart rate variability and cardiovascular disease risk factors. Int J Cardiol 141: 122–131. pmid:19910061
  30. 30. Li J, Jarczok MN, Loerbroks A, Schöllgen I, Siegrist J, et al. (2013) Work stress is associated with diabetes and prediabetes: cross-sectional results from the MIPH Industrial Cohort Studies. Int J Behav Med 20: 495–503. pmid:22915148
  31. 31. Heart rate variability: standards of measurement, physiological interpretation and clinical use. Task Force of the European Society of Cardiology and the North American Society of Pacing and Electrophysiology. (1996). Circulation 93: 1043–1065. pmid:8598068
  32. 32. Wittling A (2014) ANS Explorer http://www.neurocor.de/produkte_ansexplorer_de.html. Available: http://www.neurocor.de/produkte_ansexplorer_de.html. Accessed 31 March 2014.
  33. 33. Li J, Loerbroks A, Jarczok MN, Schöllgen I, Bosch JA, et al. (2012) Psychometric properties and differential explanation of a short measure of effort-reward imbalance at work: a study of industrial workers in Germany. Am J Ind Med 55: 808–815. pmid:22298287
  34. 34. Jenkins CD, Stanton BA, Niemcryk SJ, Rose RM (1988) A scale for the estimation of sleep problems in clinical research. J Clin Epidemiol 41: 313–321. pmid:3351539
  35. 35. Alberti KGMM, Eckel RH, Grundy SM, Zimmet PZ, Cleeman JI, et al. (2009) Harmonizing the metabolic syndrome: a joint interim statement of the International Diabetes Federation Task Force on Epidemiology and Prevention; National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute; American Heart Association; World Heart Federation; International. Circulation 120: 1640–1645. pmid:19805654
  36. 36. Janszky I, Lekander M, Blom M, Georgiades A, Ahnve S (2005) Self-rated health and vital exhaustion, but not depression, is related to inflammation in women with coronary heart disease. Brain Behav Immun 19: 555–563. pmid:16214026
  37. 37. Thayer JF, Fischer JE (2013) Heart rate variability, overnight urinary norepinephrine, and plasma cholesterol in apparently healthy human adults. Int J Cardiol 162: 240–244. pmid:21641664
  38. 38. Jarczok MN, Koenig J, Schuster AK, Thayer JF, Fischer JE (2013) Nighttime heart rate variability, overnight urinary norepinephrine, and glycemic status in apparently healthy human adults. Int J Cardiol 168: 3025–3026. pmid:23651814
  39. 39. Hillebrand S, Gast KB, de Mutsert R, Swenne C a, Jukema JW, et al. (2013) Heart rate variability and first cardiovascular event in populations without known cardiovascular disease: meta-analysis and dose-response meta-regression. Europace 15: 742–749. pmid:23370966
  40. 40. Jarczok MN, Jarczok M, Mauss D, Koenig J, Li J, et al. (2013) Autonomic nervous system activity and workplace stressors—a systematic review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 37: 1810–1823. pmid:23891906
  41. 41. Kemp AH, Quintana DS, Gray M a, Felmingham KL, Brown K, et al. (2010) Impact of depression and antidepressant treatment on heart rate variability: a review and meta-analysis. Biol Psychiatry 67: 1067–1074. pmid:20138254
  42. 42. Thayer JF, Loerbroks A, Sternberg EM (2011) Inflammation and cardiorespiratory control: the role of the vagus nerve. Respir Physiol Neurobiol 178: 387–394. pmid:21642019
  43. 43. Groves D a, Brown VJ (2005) Vagal nerve stimulation: a review of its applications and potential mechanisms that mediate its clinical effects. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 29: 493–500. pmid:15820552
  44. 44. Koenig J, Jarczok MN, Ellis RJ, Hillecke TK, Thayer JF (2013) Heart rate variability and experimentally induced pain in healthy adults: A systematic review. Eur J Pain 18: 301–314. .
  45. 45. Thayer JF, Sternberg EM (2010) Neural aspects of immunomodulation: focus on the vagus nerve. Brain Behav Immun 24: 1223–1228. pmid:20674737
  46. 46. Luft FC (2012) Neural regulation of the immune system modulates hypertension-induced target-organ damage. J Am Soc Hypertens 6: 23–26. pmid:22047671
  47. 47. Sternberg EM (2006) Neural regulation of innate immunity: a coordinated nonspecific host response to pathogens. Nat Rev Immunol 6: 318–328. pmid:16557263
  48. 48. Tracey KJ (2010) Understanding immunity requires more than immunology. Nat Immunol 11: 561–564. pmid:20562838
  49. 49. Kissler HJ, Hauffen J, Hennig R, Gepp H, Schwille PO (2005) Glucose and lipid metabolism after liver transplantation in inbred rats: consequences of hepatic denervation. Metabolism 54: 881–890. pmid:15988696
  50. 50. Marty N, Dallaporta M, Thorens B (2007) Brain glucose sensing, counterregulation, and energy homeostasis. Physiology (Bethesda) 22: 241–251. pmid:17699877
  51. 51. Pocai A, Obici S, Schwartz GJ, Rossetti L (2005) A brain-liver circuit regulates glucose homeostasis. Cell Metab 1: 53–61. pmid:16054044
  52. 52. Henderson L a, Gandevia SC, Macefield VG (2007) Somatotopic organization of the processing of muscle and cutaneous pain in the left and right insula cortex: a single-trial fMRI study. Pain 128: 20–30. pmid:17011704
  53. 53. Weiss T, Straube T, Boettcher J, Hecht H, Spohn D, et al. (2008) Brain activation upon selective stimulation of cutaneous C- and Adelta-fibers. Neuroimage 41: 1372–1381. pmid:18499480
  54. 54. Pereira E a C, Green AL, Bradley KM, Soper N, Moir L, et al. (2007) Regional cerebral perfusion differences between periventricular grey, thalamic and dual target deep brain stimulation for chronic neuropathic pain. Stereotact Funct Neurosurg 85: 175–183. pmid:17389817
  55. 55. Wagner DC, Short JL (2014) Longitudinal predictors of self-rated health and mortality in older adults. Prev Chronic Dis 11: E93. pmid:24901793
  56. 56. Kim J-H, Kim KR, Cho KH, Yoo K-B, Kwon JA, et al. (2013) The association between sleep duration and self-rated health in the Korean general population. J Clin Sleep Med 9: 1057–1064. pmid:24127150
  57. 57. Geiger SD, Sabanayagam C, Shankar A (2012) The relationship between insufficient sleep and self-rated health in a nationally representative sample. J Environ Public Health 2012: 518263. pmid:22666278
  58. 58. Hall M, Vasko R, Buysse D, Ombao H, Chen Q, et al. (2004) Acute Stress Affects Heart Rate Variability During Sleep. Psychosom Med 66: 56–62. pmid:14747638
  59. 59. Lane RD, Wager TD (2009) The new field of Brain-Body Medicine: what have we learned and where are we headed? Neuroimage 47: 1135–1140. pmid:19524045
  60. 60. Page KA, Seo D, Belfort-DeAguiar R, Lacadie C, Dzuira J, et al. (2011) Circulating glucose levels modulate neural control of desire for high-calorie foods in humans. J Clin Invest 121: 4161–4169. pmid:21926468
  61. 61. Page KA, Chan O, Arora J, Belfort-Deaguiar R, Dzuira J, et al. (2013) Effects of fructose vs glucose on regional cerebral blood flow in brain regions involved with appetite and reward pathways. JAMA J Am Med Assoc 309: 63–70. .
  62. 62. Thayer JF, Lane RD (2009) Claude Bernard and the heart-brain connection: further elaboration of a model of neurovisceral integration. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 33: 81–88. pmid:18771686
  63. 63. Roy M, Shohamy D, Wager TD (2012) Ventromedial prefrontal-subcortical systems and the generation of affective meaning. Trends Cogn Sci 16: 147–156. pmid:22310704
  64. 64. Cameron OG (2001) Interoception: the inside story—a model for psychosomatic processes. Psychosom Med 63: 697–710. pmid:11573016
  65. 65. Thayer JF, Hansen AL, Saus-Rose E, Johnsen BH (2009) Heart rate variability, prefrontal neural function, and cognitive performance: the neurovisceral integration perspective on self-regulation, adaptation, and health. Ann Behav Med 37: 141–153. pmid:19424767
  66. 66. Thayer JF, Friedman BH (2004) A Neurovisceral Integration Model of Health Disparities in Aging. In: Anderson NB, Bulatao RA, Cohen B, editors. Critical Perspectives on Racial and Ethnic Differences in Health in Late Life. Washington (DC): National Academies Press (US). pp. 567–603.
  67. 67. Thayer JF, Lane RD (2000) A model of neurovisceral integration in emotion regulation and dysregulation. J Affect Disord 61: 201–216. pmid:11163422
  68. 68. Thayer JF, Sternberg E (2006) Beyond heart rate variability: vagal regulation of allostatic systems. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1088: 361–372. pmid:17192580
  69. 69. Thayer JF, Friedman BH (2002) Stop that! Inhibition, sensitization, and their neurovisceral concomitants. Scand J Psychol 43: 123–130. pmid:12004949
  70. 70. Benyamini Y, Gerber Y, Molshatzki N, Goldbourt U, Drory Y (2014) Recovery of self-rated health as a predictor of recurrent ischemic events after first myocardial infarction: a 13-year follow-up. Health Psychol 33: 317–325. pmid:23437851
  71. 71. Benyamini Y, Blumstein T, Murad H, Lerner-Geva L (2011) Changes over time from baseline poor self-rated health: for whom does poor self-rated health not predict mortality? Psychol Health 26: 1446–1462. pmid:22011289
  72. 72. Dowd JB, Zajacova A (2010) Does self-rated health mean the same thing across socioeconomic groups? Evidence from biomarker data. Ann Epidemiol 20: 743–749. pmid:20816313
  73. 73. Mäntyselkä PT, Turunen JHO, Ahonen RS, Kumpusalo E a (2003) Chronic pain and poor self-rated health. JAMA 290: 2435–2442. pmid:14612480