Elsevier

Neuropsychologia

Volume 48, Issue 7, June 2010, Pages 2041-2050
Neuropsychologia

Children's executive functions: Are they poorer after very early brain insult

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.03.025Get rights and content

Abstract

Traditionally early brain insult (EBI) has been considered to have better outcome than later injury, consistent with the notion that the young brain is flexible and able to reorganize. Recent research findings question this view, suggesting that EBI might lead to poorer outcome than brain insult at any other age. Exploring this early vulnerability perspective, we investigated whether skills developing later in childhood, for example, executive functions (EF), would be at greater risk of disruption from EBI. The aim of this study was to investigate EF in children sustaining EBI at different developmental stages. We expected that brain insult during gestation and infancy, before the emergence of EF, would lead to global EF deficits. In contrast, we predicted that brain injury in late childhood would have fewer consequences. Using a cross-sectional, retrospective, group design we compared six groups of children (Total N = 164), with a history of brain insult and documented focal brain pathology, aged 10–16 years on a range of measures of EF. Groups were based on age of EBI: (1) Congenital; (2) Peri-natal; (3) Infancy; (4) Preschool; (5) Middle Childhood; and (6) Late Childhood. Children with EBI were at increased risk for impairment across all aspects of EF. Presence of seizures and/or frontal pathology were not predictive of outcome, but age at insult was. Children sustaining EBI before age 3 recorded more global and severe EF deficits, while children with later EBI performed closer to normal expectations. With the exception of attentional control, skills emerging at time of insult were found to be more vulnerable to disruption than those previously established, supporting the ‘early vulnerability’ model for EBI.

Introduction

While, traditionally, the young brain has been thought to possess a high level of ‘plasticity’ with a greater capacity for regeneration and recovery than that of the older child or adult brain, the relative advantage that this provides, if any, is unclear (Ballantyne et al., 2007, Ballantyne et al., 2008, Giza and Prins, 2006, Hebb, 1942, Hebb, 1949, Huttenlocher and Dabholkar, 1997, Johnston, 2009, Kennard, 1936, Kennard, 1940). In fact, recent research suggests that the young brain may be uniquely ‘vulnerable’ to insult, and that, if damaged at a critical stage of development, cognitive skills dependent on that region may be irreversibly impaired (Anderson and Moore, 1995, Anderson et al., 1997, Bittigau et al., 2004, Luciana, 2003).

Plasticity perspectives are underpinned by the assumption that the young brain is less functionally committed than that of the older child or adult brain, and so skills thought to be subsumed by damaged brain regions may be more easily transferred or reorganized as a result. This view derives from early animal and human studies, which documented better recovery from brain insult in infants than adults (Kennard, 1936, Kennard, 1940, Lenneberg, 1967, Woods, 1980). In contrast, early vulnerability theories see this lack of specialization as a disadvantage, suggesting that the young injured brain may recover in ways not specified by the normal developmental blueprint, thus leading to anomalous results (Giza and Prins, 2006, Hebb, 1942, Hebb, 1949). In addition, reduced healthy brain tissue may well lead to a ‘crowding effect, which acts to depress all functional abilities (Lansdell, 1969, Satz et al., 1994).

Outcome studies from both animal and clinical domains have investigated these two conflicting perspectives (Anderson and Moore, 1995, Anderson et al., 2004, Aram and Eisele, 1994, Bates et al., 2001, Giza and Prins, 2006, Jacobs et al., 2007, Kolb et al., 2004, Pavlovic et al., 2006, Stiles et al., 2008). Findings suggest that, while extent and location of injury are likely to predict severity of residual impairments, the timing of early brain insult (EBI) will determine the nature of these impairments, with skills already established being relatively spared, but those emerging or partially developed at risk of disruption, which may lead to either transient or more persisting sequelae (Dennis, 1989, Johnson, 2005, Thomas and Johnson, 2008). While there is little empirical evidence available, it would be expected that very early brain insult, for example, during the first trimester when processes such as cell differentiation are occurring, would have the most global and dramatic effects on functional development, Such early insults would potentially lead to disruption of cell characteristics and neural and network connections, as well as functional organization (Johnson, 2001, Johnson, 2005, Thomas and Johnson, 2008). To study the impact of age at insult, it is necessary to compare outcomes associated with brain insults sustained at different time points while the brain is in a rapid state of development, for skills which demonstrate developmental trajectories across this timeframe. Executive functions, which demonstrate protracted development from infancy to late adolescence provide such a model, and are the focus of this paper.

Executive functions (EF) are skills necessary for purposeful, goal directed activity. Components of cognitive aspects of EF include attentional control, cognitive flexibility/working memory, goal setting and processing speed (Anderson et al., 2001a, Fuster, 1993, Stuss and Benson, 1986), while behavioral aspects include emotional control, self-monitoring, initiative and insight (Stuss & Anderson, 2004). Deficits in EF may interfere with the child's capacity to develop normally and interact effectively with the environment, leading to ongoing cognitive, academic, behavioral and social disturbance (Anderson and Catroppa, 2005, De Luca et al., 2003, Dennis, 1989, Levin and Hanten, 2005). Adult literature demonstrates that EFs are largely mediated by the frontal and prefrontal cortices of the brain (Damasio, Grabowski, Frank, Galaburda, & Damasio, 1994: Stuss & Alexander, 2000), which are relatively immature during childhood, with development continuing into adolescence (Casey et al., 2000, Giedd et al., 1999, Gogtay et al., 2004, Klingberg et al., 1999, Sowell et al., 2003). Whether the same relationship between frontal regions and EF exists in the context of EBI is less clearly defined. Recent evidence suggests that executive deficits are present following EBI, regardless of lesion location (Anderson et al., 2005b, Chilosi et al., 2005, Duchowny et al., 1996, Hertz-Pannier et al., 2002, Jacobs and Anderson, 2002), likely reflecting the lack of functional specificity in the developing brain. This lack of location specificity is best established in the behavioral domain, where impairments have been reported regardless of site of lesion or timing of insult (Anderson et al., 2009, Max et al., 2004). Of note, findings from these studies should be interpreted with caution, as most employed quite a wide age range, are unable to comment whether the reported lack of location specificity is stable or changes through childhood depending on age at insult.

Parallels between ongoing maturation of the frontal lobes and executive capacities have been reported in a number of studies. Attentional control, incorporating the ability to inhibit prepotent responses, or to suppress impulsive responses, is the earliest EF domain to emerge, with the capacity to inhibit responses apparent by 7–12 months (Diamond, 2002, McKay et al., 1994, Ruff and Rothbart, 1996). By age three children can inhibit instinctive behaviors (Diamond, 2002, Espy, 1997), and improvements in impulse control continue until around age nine (Anderson, 1998). Cognitive flexibility and working memory relate to the capacity to hold information in mind and shift attention from one aspect of a stimulus to another in a flexible, efficient manner. Working memory skills begin to emerge in infancy, and consolidate in middle to late childhood (Anderson et al., 1996, Anderson et al., 2001a, De Luca et al., 2003, Luciana, 2003), while cognitive flexibility begins to come on line at around age three (Espy, 1997, Espy, 2004, Jacques and Zelazo, 2001, Zelazo et al., 2004, Smidt et al., 2004, Stuss and Anderson, 2004). The capacity for mental flexibility is established by around eight years (Anderson, 2002, Anderson et al., 2001b, Jacobs et al., 2001). Goal setting skills, the ability to plan, organize and think strategically, emerge later, but show continued maturation from middle childhood through late adolescence (Anderson et al., 1996, Anderson et al., 2001a, De Luca et al., 2003, Krikorian et al., 1994, Welsh et al., 1991), while processing speed shows gradual increments through childhood (Kail, 1988). These varying EF developmental trajectories provide an opportunity to examine the impact of brain insult, when both brain and cognition are rapidly developing.

We addressed the ‘plasticity-vulnerability’ debate by posing the following main hypothesis: Neurobehavioral recovery following EBI is not consistent with early plasticity views. We predicted that: (1) EBI would have long-term implications for EF; (2) in contrast to adult findings, as described above, location of lesion (that is, frontal or extrafrontal) would not be directly associated with poorer EF function, reflecting lack of functional specificity in the developing brain; and (3) age at brain insult would have long-term implications for EF. We expected that for cognitive aspects of EF, profiles would differ according to both age at insult and level of EF development, with younger age at insult and less established EF linked to poorer long-term outcomes. Specifically: (a) attentional control, which emerges during early childhood and is established in middle childhood, would be poorest in children with early insult (< age 3 years), and intact for those with later insults (>7 years); (b) cognitive flexibility, working memory, developing rapidly between preschool and middle childhood (3–8 years), would be poorest for those sustaining insults prior to age 3, and best for those with lesions after age 9 years; (c) goal setting, emerging in late childhood, would be poor across all early insult groups with the exception of those with insults after age 10 years; (d) processing speed, which improves gradually through childhood, would be impaired across all age groups, but with a trend for better results with older age at insult; and (e) everyday EF would be similarly reduced regardless of age at insult, due to its multi-determined nature.

Section snippets

Participants

The sample comprised 164 children (92 males) with a history of EBI, recruited from 2005 to 2007, through the Royal Children's Hospital, Melbourne, Australia. Children were aged 10–16 years at recruitment (M = 13.07, SD = 1.88). Inclusion criteria were: (1) aged 10–16 at assessment; (2) evidence of focal brain pathology, on MRI scan; and (3) brain insult >12 months prior to assessment. Exclusion criteria were: (1) diffuse pathology (e.g., closed head injury) on MRI scan; and (2) non-English

Sample demographics

No group differences were identified for gender or SES. There was a significant age at test effect, F(5,158) = 3.21, p = .009, η2 = .09, with the LC group older than the CON, INF, PRE and MC groups (all p < .05). Group differences were also present for age at diagnosis, F(5,149) = 64.25, p < .001, η2 = .68, and time since diagnosis, F(5,149) = 39.95, p < .001, η2 = .57, in the expected direction. History of seizures also differed across groups, χ2 (5, N = 158) = 17.44, p = .004, V = .332, and was more likely with earlier

Discussion

This study aimed to contribute to the early plasticity-early vulnerability debate by exploring EF outcomes after EBI. Children sustaining EBI during six developmental periods, from gestation to late childhood, and with focal brain pathology evident on MRI scan, were compared across a range of EF domains: attentional control, cognitive flexibility/working memory, goal setting, processing speed and everyday EF. Assessments were conducted during late childhood/adolescence, when maturational

Conclusions

Results from this study provide support for the ‘early vulnerability model’ for EBI. Compared to population expectations, children with EBI were at increased risk for impairment across all aspects of EF—attentional control, cognitive flexibility/working memory, goal setting, processing speed and everyday EF. Further, and inconsistent with previous research, presence of seizures and/or frontal consistent predictive of EF outcomes. In contrast, age at insult was associated with EF outcomes. In

Acknowledgements

This research was supported by an Australian Research Council Linkage Grant: Developmental lesions and cognitive outcomes. (Anderson, Leventer, Jacobs).

References (81)

  • D. Stuss et al.

    The frontal lobes and theory of mind: Developmental concepts from adult focal lesion research

    Brain and Cognition

    (2004)
  • B. Woods

    The restricted effects of right hemisphere lesions after age one: Weschsler test data

    Neuropsychologia

    (1980)
  • P. Zelazo et al.

    Executive function across the life span

    Acta Psychologia

    (2004)
  • P. Anderson

    Assessment and development of executive function (EF) during childhood

    Child Neuropsychology

    (2002)
  • P. Anderson et al.

    Assessment and development of organisational ability: The Rey Complex Figure organisational strategy score (RCF-OSS)

    The Clinical Neuropsychologist

    (2001)
  • P. Anderson et al.

    The Tower of London Test: Validation and standardization for pediatric populations

    The Clinical Neuropsychologist

    (1996)
  • V. Anderson

    Assessment of executive function in children

    Neuropsychological Rehabilitation

    (1998)
  • V. Anderson et al.

    Development of executive functions through late childhood and adolescence in and Australian sample

    Developmental Neuropsychology

    (2001)
  • V. Anderson et al.

    Childhood bacterial meningitis: Impact of age at illness and medical complications on long-term outcome

    Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society

    (1997)
  • V. Anderson et al.

    Recovery of executive skills following pediatric traumatic brain injury (TBI): A two year follow-up

    Brain Injury

    (2005)
  • V. Anderson et al.

    Functional plasticity or vulnerability following early brain injury?

    Pediatrics

    (2005)
  • V. Anderson et al.

    Prefrontal lesions and attentional skills in childhood

    Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society

    (2005)
  • V. Anderson et al.

    Age at injury as a predictor of outcome following pediatric head injury

    Child Neuropsychology

    (1995)
  • V. Anderson et al.

    Thirty-month outcome from early childhood head injury: A prospective analysis of neurobehavioral recovery

    Brain

    (2004)
  • V. Anderson et al.

    Childhood brain insult: Can age at insult help us predict outcome?

    Brain

    (2009)
  • A.O. Ballantyne et al.

    Plasticity in the developing brain: intellectual, language and academic functions in children with ischaemic perinatal stroke

    Brain, 131.

    (2008)
  • A.O. Ballantyne et al.

    Language outcome after perinatal stroke: does side matter?

    Child Neuropsychology

    (2007)
  • A.M. Chilosi et al.

    Atypical language lateralization and early linguistic development in children with focal brain lesions

    Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology

    (2005)
  • A. Diamond

    Normal development of prefrontal cortex from birth to young adulthood: Cognitive functions, anatomy, and biochemistry

  • A. Damasio et al.

    The return of Phineas Gage: clues about the brain from the skull of a famous patient

    Science

    (1994)
  • A. Daniel

    Power, privilege and prestige: Occupations in Australia

    (1983)
  • D. Delis et al.

    Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System (D-KEFS)

    (2001)
  • C. De Luca et al.

    Normative data from the CANTAB: Development of executive function over the lifespan

    Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology

    (2003)
  • M. Dennis

    Language and the young damaged brain

  • M. Dennis et al.

    Hy IQ is not a covariate in cognitive studies of neurodevelopmental disorders

    Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society

    (2009)
  • M. Duchowny et al.

    Language cortex representation: effects of developmental versus acquired pathology

    Annals of Neurology

    (1996)
  • K.A. Espy

    The Shape School: Assessing executive function in preschool children

    Developmental Neuropsychology

    (1997)
  • K. Espy

    Using developmental, cognitive and neuroscience approaches to understand executive control in young children

    Developmental Neuropsychology

    (2004)
  • P. Fastenau et al.

    Neuropsychological status at seizure onset in children: Risk factors for early cognitive deficits

    Neurology

    (2009)
  • J. Garth et al.

    Executive functions following moderate to severe frontal lobe injury: Impact of injury and age at injury

    Pediatric Rehabilitation

    (1997)
  • Cited by (92)

    • Developmental divergence of structural brain networks as an indicator of future cognitive impairments in childhood brain injury: Executive functions

      2020, Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience
      Citation Excerpt :

      Despite these findings, divergence from the age-appropriate structural covariance in the CEN was not associated with later EF. Overall, our findings support previous conclusions that the integrity of development in the entire brain is necessary for achieving age-appropriate, intact EF (Anderson et al., 2010), rather than early vulnerability due to specific damage to the networks that subsume EF development (Anderson, 2002). Taken together, these findings underscore the importance of considering metrics of connectivity when attempting to understand how brain insults impact on functional outcomes in a developmental context.

    • Age-dependent differences in the impact of paediatric traumatic brain injury on executive functions: A prospective study using susceptibility-weighted imaging

      2019, Neuropsychologia
      Citation Excerpt :

      For children with TBI, this ‘maturation’ effect was not as apparent or pronounced. This finding is in line with the sensitive period model stating that impact during a period of rapid neural network development has detrimental consequences for the maturation of the cognitive functions underlying these developing networks (Anderson et al., 2011, 2010; Dennis et al., 2014). The emergence of this effect only at the 2-year assessment corresponds with the notion that after paediatric brain injury, deficits in higher-order skills may emerge later in development when these skills are expected to be at a stage of becoming established and consolidated (Giza and Prins, 2006; Middleton, 2001).

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text