Relations between executive function and academic achievement from ages 5 to 17 in a large, representative national sample

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2011.01.007Get rights and content

Abstract

This study examined age-related changes in complex executive function (EF) in a large, representative sample (N = 2036) aged 5 to 17 using the Cognitive Assessment System (CAS; Naglieri & Das, 1997a). Relations between complex EF and academic achievement were examined on a sub-sample (N = 1395) given the Woodcock–Johnson Tests of Achievement–Revised (Woodcock & Johnson, 1989). Performance on the three complex EF tasks improved until at least age 15, although improvement slowed with increasing age and varied some across tasks. Moreover, the different developmental patterns in the correlations between completion time and accuracy provide clues to developmental processes. Examination of individual achievement subtests clarified the specific aspects of academic performance most related to complex EF. Finally, the correlation between complex EF and academic achievement varied across ages, but the developmental pattern of the strength of these correlations was remarkably similar for overall math and reading achievement, suggesting a domain-general relation between complex EF and academic achievement.

Research Highlights

►Current study utilized a large, nationally-represented sample of children. ►Complex EF developed significantly until at least age 15. ►Metacognition appears to aid EF development. ►Both domain-general relations and intra-domain variability to achievement were discovered.

Introduction

A number of studies have focused on the origins of adaptive, goal-directed behavior—commonly referred to as executive function (EF)—in young children (see Garon, Bryson, & Smith, 2008, for a review of early EF development). During early childhood, children develop the ability to ignore distraction (Klenberg, Korkman, & Lahti-Nuuttila, 2001), inhibit prepotent and inappropriate responses (e.g., Carlson & Moses, 2001), shift between different sets of tasks (Zelazo et al., 2003a, Zelazo et al., 2003b), and then integrate these abilities to solve more complex problems (Asato et al., 2004, Bull et al., 2004, Miyake et al., 2000).

Although these early milestones of EF are important to document, fewer studies examine EF development and its correlates in middle childhood and adolescence (Best, Miller, & Jones, 2009). This later development is highly important, too, as EF is associated with school success in middle (Blair and Diamond, 2008, Blair and Razza, 2007) and late childhood (Sikora et al., 2002, van der Sluis et al., 2007). What is missing in this literature is a comprehensive look at the relations between EF and academic achievement over a wide age range. The specific relations may vary from one age to another, as EF may be more important during some phases of development than others. The present study is more comprehensive than previous studies in that it included a large sample (N = 2036), a wide age range (5 to 17), three EF tasks, nine academic tests (Woodcock–Johnson Tests of Achievement–Revised [WJ-R], Woodcock & Johnson, 1989), and several aspects of performance (accuracy, completion time, and their ratio) on the EF tasks. Moreover, unlike most studies of EF in children, we used an assessment of EF with strong psychometric properties (Cognitive Assessment System [CAS], Naglieri & Das, 1997a). Strong reliability, in particular, is important when examining correlations between tests.

Another reason for using the CAS was that it contains “complex” EF tasks that involve several components of EF and often require the coordination of those components. These three complex EF tasks comprise the CAS Planning scale. “Simple” EF tasks, conversely, attempt to isolate the EF components. (Note: There are strong arguments, however, that the EF components, particularly working memory and inhibition, are interactive by nature and cannot be isolated in a cognitive task (Roberts & Pennington, 1996)). The CAS does contain simple EF tasks within another scale, the Attention scale, and a similar analysis of those tasks can be found in Lehman, Naglieri, and Aquilino (2010). Because reading and math are complex skills, their reported correlations with EF likely reflect complex EF skills such as selecting and coordinating several EF components. Finally, we focus on complex EF based on evidence for protracted development through late adolescence (Romine & Reynolds, 2005).

In addition to examining the relations between EF and academic achievement over a broad age range, we had a second goal, of documenting the form of EF development (e.g., the magnitude of change at different ages), with identical or nearly identical tasks employed across the sample. This examination is important for a) clarifying age differences in EF using a large sample covering a wide age range, and b) identifying aspects of EF development (e.g., accuracy, time to completion) that might underlie age differences in the relations between EF and school performance, which could guide future research on this question.

Based on previous research, we expected EF to improve through the elementary school years and adolescence, though perhaps more gradually during adolescence (Davidson et al., 2006, Huizinga et al., 2006, Huizinga and van der Molen, 2007, Luciana et al., 2005, Somsen, 2007, and see Romine & Reynolds, 2005 for a meta-analysis of EF developmental studies and Best & Miller, 2010, for a review). These behavioral findings align with both structural (e.g., Gogtay et al., 2004) and functional imaging studies (e.g., Casey et al., 1997, Durston et al., 2006) reporting a protracted development of the neural substrate supporting EF.

We examined response time and accuracy separately on the EF tasks in an attempt to provide a more fine-grained analysis of EF development and to seek clues to the developmental processes involved. For example, metacognition—monitoring one's performance and adjusting behavior as needed—appears to be an important mechanism related to EF during the school years (e.g., Crone et al., 2006, Davidson et al., 2006, Somsen, 2007). One way to detect the influence of metacognition is to compare age-related changes in accuracy and reaction time on a task. In one study, Davidson, Amso, Anderson and Diamond (2006) found that both reaction time and accuracy on EF tasks increased from middle childhood to early adulthood, suggesting a speed-accuracy trade-off: Older participants adjusted their reaction times in order to maintain a high level of accuracy, which suggests the influence of metacognition on the development of mature task performance. Note that such developmental patterns suggesting the underlying developmental processes would be less evident when only narrow age ranges are tested.

There is ongoing debate about the nature of the EF construct, but one prominent theoretical framework suggests that EF constitutes distinct, yet related, components, with inhibition, updating of working memory, and shifting being foundational components (Friedman et al., 2008, Huizinga et al., 2006, Lehto et al., 2003, Miyake et al., 2000, van der Sluis et al., 2007). Based on this multi-componential framework, complex EF tasks, like those used in the present study, likely require some combination of, and coordination of, these EF components (Anderson, 2002, Asato et al., 2004, Bull et al., 2004, Huizinga and van der Molen, 2007, Huizinga et al., 2006, Miyake et al., 2000). The CAS Planning scale contains three such tasks: Matching Numbers, Planned Codes, and Planned Connections. In developing these tasks, EF was operationalized as the ability to prepare multiple steps of action in advance, evaluate those actions (updating of working memory), avoid or suppress non-goal behavior (inhibition), and change course of action if necessary (shifting) (Naglieri, 2005, Naglieri and Das, 2005). On Matching Numbers children must use controlled searches, as opposed to automatic searches (Schneider & Shiffrin, 1977), to find two identical numbers within a row of similar numbers. Since all the numbers within a row contain similar digits and are the same length, the two identical numbers do not “pop out” but must be identified by selecting and employing a controlled search strategy (Das, Naglieri, & Kirby, 1994). Planned Codes is a variation of other substitution coding tasks (also called digit-symbol coding tasks), commonly found in intelligence batteries, which require children to fill in a matrix of incomplete codes based on a decoding key at the top of the page. However, unlike substitution coding tasks that primarily involve children's perceptual speed (e.g., Laux & Lane, 1985), the Planned Codes task does not contain prespecified instructions on how to code (e.g., left to right, top to bottom). Thus, children must consider the problem, select a coding strategy, and monitor its effectiveness, shifting to another coding strategy if necessary (Naglieri et al., 1989). Finally, Planned Connections resembles the classic neuropsychological assessment of frontal lobe functioning, the Trail Making Test (e.g., Reitan, 1971). It requires children to keep a number or letter in mind to find the next number or letter and to shift between executing number and letter searches (Naglieri et al., 1989). As this description indicates, although “Planning” is the umbrella label given to these three tasks, they assess core EF components in the context of achieving task goals.

Longitudinal research suggests that EF contributes to academic achievement rather than vice versa (e.g., Bull et al., 2008, George and Greenfield, 2005, Hitch et al., 2001, Miller and Hinshaw, 2010). Furthermore, EF has been linked to academic achievement in children of various ages with and without specific learning disabilities (see Best et al., 2009, Müller et al., 2008, for reviews). Performance on inhibition and working memory tasks, in particular, consistently relates to performance in mathematics and reading (Blair and Razza, 2007, Bull and Scerif, 2001, Protopapas et al., 2007, St. Clair-Thompson and Gathercole, 2006, van der Schoot et al., 2000, van der Sluis et al., 2007). Shifting, on the other hand, does not consistently relate to academic achievement (Espy et al., 2004, van der Sluis et al., 2007).

As suggested earlier, complex EF tasks that require the coordination of the foundational EFs and the execution and monitoring of a complex sequence of actions should be of particular importance to academic achievement. Only a few studies have examined the link between complex EF and academic achievement, and most of these studies have used the Tower of London (TOL) or Tower of Hanoi (TOH) task. In both tower tasks, children must select and execute a sequence of moves in order to transform an initial pattern of balls located on pegs to a target pattern in a minimum number of moves. In one study (Bull et al., 2008), TOL performance in preschool predicted improvements in both reading and math from age 5 to age 8. The authors reason that early complex EF skills are domain-general rather than domain-specific skills that provide the building blocks for the development of math and reading skills. Another study (Altemeier, Jones, Abbott, & Berninger, 2006) suggests a more nuanced relationship. After controlling for lower-level EF performance, performance on a modified TOH task uniquely predicted the ability to translate previously-taken notes into a report in 3rd graders, but not 5th graders. Tower performance, however, did not uniquely predict the ability to take notes from a written passage in either grade level. The authors suggested that in younger children, report-writing is less automatic and requires more effortful planning and coordination, but it is unclear why complex EF was not a unique contributor to note-taking skills.

In another study (Sikora et al., 2002), children (aged 7–18) with arithmetic difficulties exhibited greater TOL impairment than children with reading difficulties or children with no diagnosed academic difficulty. Although Sikora et al. did not offer any explanations as to why TOL performance would be more closely linked to math performance, they did suggest that the cognitive processes needed for math may differ from those needed for reading. Finally, Cohen, Bronson, and Casey (1995) found that two complex EF tasks (the TOH and Trail-Making task) did not predict 3rd graders' general school performance (as indexed by a composite of reading, language arts, and math grades) but did not examine each school subject separately.

Together, these studies suggest that complex EF is linked to academic performance in some way; however, differences in sample and task characteristics across studies prohibit drawing strong conclusions. Thus, at least two uncertainties remain: (1) Previous research is inconsistent concerning whether EF is domain-general and needed in various academic skills, or domain-specific and related differentially to academic skills. (2) There is some evidence that EF is more closely related to academic achievement in early schooling (Altemeier et al., 2006), although no study has yet to examine this relationship across all of primary and secondary schooling. Thus, an investigation of the link between EF and academic achievement, both reading and math, in a large and representative sample with a broad age span should clarify this topic.

In the present study the standardization sample from the CAS was used to examine the development of EF across a broad, continuous age range (5 to 17 years) and relations between EF and academic achievement. This sample is ideal for several reasons. First, the exceptionally wide age range permits a more complete description of EF development than previous studies and thus may a) identify periods of rapid change, and b) clarify the relation between EF and academic performance. Second, the large sample size (N = 2036) increases the power of detecting subtle changes with age and permits a detailed description of EF development. Third, unlike in most EF studies, the sample matches the U.S. population on important demographic variables, including gender, race, ethnicity, parental education, geographic region, and community setting. Thus, the ability to generalize the results of this sample is maximized. Finally, a subset of the children (N = 1395) also completed the WJ-R, permitting an investigation of the association between EF and academic achievement—both math and reading—across development.

Beyond the benefits of this sample, the CAS has many characteristics that enrich the study of EF development. In particular, it meets recommendations delineated by Berg and his colleagues (Berg and Byrd, 2002, Berg et al., 2006) for the TOL, a commonly used complex EF task. The two main recommendations are that EF researchers develop standardized versions of EF tasks and that assessments include multiple performance indices, including at least one accuracy measure and one speed measure. Being a standardized task, the CAS allows researchers to make comparisons across age groups and testing sites. The CAS also contains several performance indices, including completion time and accuracy. We thus can examine the convergence of the results and, specifically, determine whether completion time and accuracy show similar developmental trends and whether they are related to each other (e.g., indicating a speed–accuracy tradeoff).

Our first aim is to examine the form of complex EF development across middle childhood and adolescence. The complex EF tests on the CAS for younger (5–7) and older (8–17) children are comprised of the same types of items, but in order to avoid floor and ceiling effects, the CAS gives them different time limits and/or different sets of items. This configuration interferes with the direct comparison of development between ages 7 and 8 years but it allows for more precise measurement for the 5–7 and 8–17 year age ranges. Based on previous empirical work (e.g., Romine & Reynolds, 2005), we hypothesize that EF scores will continue to improve across the entire age range, but that the magnitude of those improvements will decrease with age. Also, relations between completion time and accuracy at various ages are of particular interest, especially any evidence for a speed–accuracy tradeoff, as this may reveal the influence of metacognition on task performance.

Our second aim is to examine the relations between EF and academic achievement. Based on the few studies that have examined complex EF and achievement, it is uncertain whether EF is related generally to academic achievement or more specifically to certain academic skills. Naglieri and Das (1997b) reported the correlations between the composite of the three CAS EF tasks and the WJ-R. To extend this work and provide a more fine-grained analysis of relations between EF and academic achievement, we will examine all nine subtests, with a focus on reading and math, and perform detailed analyses to explore age- and task-related differences in correlation strength.

Section snippets

Participants

Participants included children and adolescents (N = 2036; see Table 1 for N by age) between the ages of 5 and 17 years, 11 months (M = 9.4 years, SD = 3.7), who participated in the standardization of the CAS. Children from 68 sites across the United States were recruited by letters and consent forms sent home to parents from school. This sample was representative of the U.S. population (1990 census data) on the basis of gender (50% female), race (77% White, 13% African American, 10% Other), ethnicity

What is the pattern of improvement in EF with increasing age?

Means, standard deviations, and sample sizes for the 3 EF tasks are presented separately for the completion time and accuracy in Table 1, Table 2, respectively. To test EF development, MANOVA was performed on the ratio scores for each task. The omnibus test indicated differences in performance based on age for both the younger age group, Pillai's F(6,1810) = 94.90, p < .001, η2p = .24, and the older age group, Pillai's F(12,3201) = 61.16, p < .001, η2p = .19. Follow-up univariate tests determined that

Age-related changes in EF

This study provided a fine-grained analysis of age differences in complex EF by comparing the magnitude of change across adjacent age groups. Generally, the magnitude of EF improvement was large across the youngest groups, became more moderate in late childhood, and diminished further during adolescence. This finding supports, in a single study with a wide age range and three complex EF tasks, the conclusions of a meta-analysis (Romine & Reynolds, 2005), in which complex EF ability increased

References (69)

  • S. van der Sluis et al.

    Executive functioning in children, and its relations with reasoning, reading, and arithmetic

    Intelligence

    (2007)
  • A. Winsler et al.

    Children's search strategies and accompanying verbal and motor strategic behavior: Developmental trends and relations with task performance among children age 5 to 17

    Cognitive Development

    (2006)
  • L. Altemeier et al.

    Executive functions in becoming writing readers and reading writers: Note taking and report writing in third and fifth graders

    Developmental Neuropsychology

    (2006)
  • P. Anderson

    Assessment and development of executive function (EF) during childhood

    Child Neuropsychology

    (2002)
  • M.R. Asato et al.

    Cognitive processes in the development of TOL performance

    Neuropsychologia

    (2004)
  • W.K. Berg et al.

    The Tower of London spatial problem solving task: Enhancing clinical and research implementation

    Journal of Experimental and Clinical Neuropsychology

    (2002)
  • W.K. Berg et al.

    New indices of planning abilities using the Tower of London task

  • J.R. Best et al.

    A developmental perspective on executive function

    Child Development

    (2010)
  • C. Blair et al.

    Biological processes in prevention and intervention: The promotion of self-regulation as a means of preventing school failure

    Development and Psychopathology

    (2008)
  • C. Blair et al.

    Relating effortful control, executive function, and false belief understanding to emerging math and literacy ability in Kindergarten

    Child Development

    (2007)
  • R. Bull et al.

    A comparison of performance on the Towers of London and Hanoi in young children

    Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry

    (2004)
  • R. Bull et al.

    Short-term memory, working memory, and executive functioning in preschoolers: Longitudinal predictors of mathematical achievement at age 7 years

    Developmental Neuropsychology

    (2008)
  • R. Bull et al.

    Executive functioning as a predictor of children's mathematics ability: Inhibition, switching, and working memory

    Developmental Neuropsychology

    (2001)
  • S.M. Carlson et al.

    Individual differences in inhibitory control and children's theory of mind

    Child Development

    (2001)
  • B.J. Casey et al.

    A developmental functional MRI study of prefrontal activation during performance of a go-no-go task

    Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience

    (1997)
  • J. Cohen

    Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences

    (1988)
  • G.N. Cohen et al.

    Planning as a factor in school achievement

    Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology

    (1995)
  • J.P. Das et al.

    Assessment of cognitive processes: The PASS theory of intelligence

    (1994)
  • A. Diamond et al.

    Preschool program improves cognitive control

    Science

    (2007)
  • A. Diamond et al.

    The early development of executive functions

  • S. Durston et al.

    A shift from diffuse to focal cortical activity with development

    Developmental Science

    (2006)
  • K.A. Espy et al.

    The contribution of executive functions to emergent mathematic skills in preschool children

    Developmental Neuropsychology

    (2004)
  • N.P. Friedman et al.

    Individual differences in executive functions are almost entirely genetic in origin

    Journal of Experimental Psychology-General

    (2008)
  • N. Garon et al.

    Executive function in preschoolers: A review using an integrative framework

    Psychological Bulletin

    (2008)
  • Cited by (838)

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text