Original article
Newborns at risk for special education placement: A population-based study

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ejpn.2007.01.005Get rights and content

Abstract

Objectives

To establish the contributions of birth weight (BW), gender, socioeconomic status (SES), and parental age on risks for special education (SE) placements in school-age children.

Methods

A population-based sample of 900 school-age children attending the following full-time SE groups: at level 1, children had isolated neurodevelopmental, physical, or other impairments; at level 2, borderline to mild intellectual disability (ID); and at level 3, moderate to severe ID. Three hundred and one children enrolled in mainstream education formed the control group (level 0). For all children with siblings, we defined familiar forms of learning disorders as having a sibling in one of the SE groupings. We performed our analysis for the entire cohort as well as comparing risk factors within the familial and non-familial types of SE groupings.

Results

In multinomial logistic regression analysis, age of father ⩾40 years, low BW (<2500 g or <−2 SD), male sex, and parent's lower SES, all increased the probability of SE placement. In the familial forms of levels 2 and 3, the parental SES was lower and, in addition, in the level 2, the family size was bigger. Furthermore, in the non-familial form of level 2, both the low and the high (⩾4000 g) BW were more common.

Conclusions

Among the known risk factors for learning disabilities (LD), our study highlighted the importance of a higher paternal age and a lower SES especially in the familial forms of LD.

Introduction

Learning disabilities (LD) form a heterogeneous group of disorders with a varying degree of severity ranging from dyslexia or dyscalculia to severe intellectual disability (ID). During recent years, the recognition of LDs has increased in Finland and thus the prevalence rates of LDs have risen. For instance, in 1995, 2.9% of the pupils 7–16-years old attended full-time and 13.9% part-time special education (SE) groups, whereas in 2001, the corresponding numbers raised to 5.2% and 16%.1 These numbers match the prevalence rates of the LDs in other western countries.2, 3

The reasons for SE placement are diverse, involving both biological and environmental factors. Among these are the associations of increasing maternal and paternal age with the offspring having a lower cognitive capacity.4, 5 In addition, factors such as birth order, family size, a sibling's LD, and especially a family's low social class, may all correlate with LDs.6, 7, 8, 9, 10 Furthermore, earlier studies show that an increasing birth weight (BW) in the normal range correlates positively with favourable cognitive function during childhood and young adult life.11, 12 By contrast, low BW associates with LDs among school-aged children.2, 9, 13, 14, 15 Other potential factors for cognitive impairment are foetal growth restriction and male sex.15, 16, 17, 18

In this clearly defined population-based study, we attempted to establish how these factors at birth associate with the future need for full-time SE (Table 1). Secondly, another objective was to discover whether children with the familial (at least one sibling in SE) and non-familial subtypes of LD displayed differences and if they did, to clarify their distinctive profiles. We chose the levels of full-time SE (Table 1) as the dependent variable since they measure children's abilities globally.

Section snippets

Study scheme and randomisation

This population-based study was conducted in southern Finland, in the former county of Uusimaa during the years 1997–2002. This county includes the capital, Helsinki, and its environs, which had a population of 1.4 million, and had a gross national product per inhabitant that was 143% of the EU-15 mean.1 Uusimaa is also a diverse area of both urban and agricultural communities.

Information was gathered on pupils who were placed in full-time SE groups for neurodevelopmental reasons. In Finland,

Results

Of the children who were in the level 1 SE groups, 56% encountered problems in speech and language development and of those in the level 3 SE groups, 50% had a known aetiology for their ID (n=113 for a known syndrome or chromosomal aberration, n=7 for postnatal infections and tumours, n=7 for foetal alcohol syndrome, n=7 for metabolic or storage disease, n=2 for severe birth complications). Children in the degree 2 SE groups were, on the other hand, a very heterogeneous group with only few of

Discussion

In this population-based cross-sectional study, we evaluated the importance of factors that are present at birth to determine a future need for full-time SE. The SE groups in this study represent a continuum of IQs from specific LDs to severe intellectual disabilities. We consider the strengths of this approach to be a relatively large sample size (900 subjects and 301 controls), a clearly defined and diverse catchment-area, and the study design, which allowed us to personally collect data on

Acknowledgements

The Arvo and Lea Ylppö Foundation, the Rinnekoti Research Foundation, Espoo, and the EVO Fund of the Helsinki Medical Imaging Center, Helsinki University Central Hospital, all in Finland, supported this study.

References (28)

  • B.W. Camp et al.

    Maternal and neonatal risk factors for mental retardation: defining the “at-risk” child

    Early Hum Dev

    (1998)
  • M.V. Johnston

    Clinical disorders of brain plasticity

    Brain Dev

    (2004)
  • Central Statistical Office of Finland 2003;...
  • R.N. Avchen et al.

    Birth weight and school-age disabilities: a population-based study

    Am J Epidemiol

    (2001)
  • M.E. Msall et al.

    Functional disability and school activity limitations in 41,300 school-age children: relationship to medical impairments

    Pediatrics

    (2003)
  • M. Auroux

    Decrease of learning capacity in offspring with increasing paternal age in the rat

    Teratology

    (1983)
  • M.R. Auroux et al.

    Paternal age and mental functions of progeny in man

    Hum Reprod

    (1989)
  • L.A. Croen et al.

    The epidemiology of mental retardation of unknown cause

    Pediatrics

    (2001)
  • D. Goldberg et al.

    Which newborns in New York city are at risk for special education placement?

    Am J Public Health

    (1992)
  • M.M. Kappelman et al.

    Comparison of disadvantaged children with learning disabilities and their successful peer group

    Am J Dis Child

    (1972)
  • S. Bundey et al.

    The recurrence risks for mild idiopathic mental retardation

    J Med Genet

    (1989)
  • M. Richards et al.

    Birth weight and cognitive function in the British 1946 birth cohort: longitudinal population-based study

    Br Med J

    (2001)
  • B.J. Jefferis et al.

    Birth weight, childhood socioeconomic environment, and cognitive development in the 1958 British birth cohort study

    Br Med J

    (2002)
  • P. Anderson et al.

    Neurobehavioral outcomes of school-age children born extremely low birth weight or very preterm in the 1990s

    J Am Med Assoc

    (2003)
  • Cited by (13)

    • Childhood growth and development associated with need for full-time special education at school age

      2009, European Journal of Paediatric Neurology
      Citation Excerpt :

      To explore possible differences between the familial and non-familial LD subtypes, we performed an analysis comparing children with a sibling in SE to children without such a sibling. This work is part of our larger population-based study described earlier in detail.17 Briefly, the study group collected in random order comprised of 900 school-age children (age-range 7–16 years; mean 12.7) participating in full-time SE groups at three different levels after psychological evaluation.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text