Elsevier

Addictive Behaviors

Volume 39, Issue 11, November 2014, Pages 1547-1556
Addictive Behaviors

Impulsivity: Four ways five factors are not basic to addiction

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2014.01.002Get rights and content

Highlights

  • Impulsivity is multifaceted, but debate continues as to the precise number of facets.

  • Situation resembles debate between H. J. Eysenck and Costa & McCrae concerning personality structure.

  • Strong evidence for unique role of two factors of impulsivity in addictive behavior, weak evidence for five factors

  • Two-factor impulsivity models, anchored in biological processes, show remarkable consistency across domains.

  • Consideration of biological evidence provides a necessary constraint on theory development.

Abstract

Several impulsivity-related models have been applied to understanding the vulnerability to addiction. While there is a growing consensus that impulsivity is multifaceted, debate continues as to the precise number of facets and, more critically, which are most relevant to explaining the addiction-risk profile. In many ways, the current debate mirrors that which took place in the personality literature in the early 1990s (e.g., Eysenck's ‘Big Three’ versus Costa and McCrae's ‘Big Five’). Indeed, many elements of this debate are relevant to the current discussion of the role of impulsivity in addictive behavior. Specifically, 1) the use of factor analysis as an atheoretical ‘truth-grinding machine’; 2) whether additional facets add explanatory power over fewer; 3) the delineation of specific neurocognitive pathways from each facet to addictive behaviors, and; 4) the relative merit of ‘top-down’ versus ‘bottom-up’ approaches to the understanding of impulsivity. Ultimately, the utility of any model of impulsivity and addiction lies in its heuristic value and ability to integrate evidence from different levels of analysis. Here, we make the case that theoretically-driven, bottom-up models proposing two factors deliver the optimal balance of explanatory power, parsimony, and integration of evidence.

Introduction

Impulsivity, whether measured by self-report, observer-report, or behavioral performance, is a robust predictor of current and future problems with substance use (Dawe and Loxton, 2004, Jentsch and Taylor, 1999, Moeller et al., 2001, Moffitt et al., 2011, Nigg et al., 2006, Potenza, 2013, Tarter et al., 2003). In children, its association with future substance use remains even after controlling for other markers of risk, including low IQ, socioeconomic status, and parental history of substance dependence (Moffitt et al., 2011, Nigg et al., 2006, Tarter et al., 2003). Not surprisingly, the construct is of great interest to addiction scientists.

In addiction science, there is an emerging consensus that impulsive drug use involves two core processes observable at the neurophysiological, behavioral, cognitive, and trait levels. The first involves a heightened propensity, or impulse, to approach drugs and the second involves a reduced capacity to inhibit this approach behavior. The summary presented in Table 1 highlights the considerable overlap of different theoretical models in the importance placed on these two fundamental processes. Notably, these models have been derived from multiple researchers across diverse methodological investigations.

While a two-factor model is attractive in its parsimony, other researchers have proposed that a more useful way to consider impulsivity is to develop a more nuanced delineation of subtypes. This would have important implications for addiction science. In an attempt to “bring order to the myriad of measures and conceptions of impulsivity”, Whiteside and Lynam (2001, p. 684) drew upon the Five Factor Model of human personality (Costa and McCrae, 1992, Goldberg, 1993) as a framework for conceptualizing impulsivity. Employing factor analysis of self-report data, they constructed the four-factor UPPS impulsivity questionnaire consisting of: Urgency, (lack of) Premeditation, (lack of) Perseverance, and Sensation seeking. Subsequently, Cyders et al. (2007) argued that the UPPS model was incomplete, in that it did not incorporate impulsive behavior arising from positive mood states. They proposed that individual differences in this tendency were important to consider in understanding risky behavior such as alcohol abuse, and used factor analysis to derive an additional scale to measure the construct. Thus, the Urgency subscale was renamed Negative Urgency and a new scale added, Positive Urgency. We refer to this extended model as the UPPS + P model.

Notably, UPPS Sensation Seeking and (lack of) Premeditation align somewhat with the core processes previously implicated in impulsive substance use, and impulsivity theories more generally (Table 1). However, as the authors themselves note, “(lack of) perseverance, like urgency, is not well represented in other measures of impulsivity” (Whiteside & Lynam, 2001, p. 685C). The same could be said of Positive Urgency (Cyders et al., 2007). In debating the importance of these newly constructed impulsivity traits, the field finds itself in a situation strikingly similar to that which took place in the personality literature, in particular, the debate between Costa and McCrae (1992) and Eysenck (1992). In a paper entitled “Four ways five factors are basic”, Costa and McCrae outlined four lines of evidence to support the five-factor model of personality. This was followed by Eysenck's reply entitled, “Four ways five factors are not basic”, in which he argued against each of the proposed lines of evidence. Eysenck concluded with a strong call for a science of personality based on theoretical predictions firmly rooted in biological processes.

Many of the issues raised during the personality debate are relevant for addiction researchers studying impulsivity. Specifically, 1) the use of factor analysis as an atheoretical ‘truth-grinding’ machine; 2) whether additional facets of a construct add explanatory power over fewer; 3) the delineation of specific neurocognitive pathways from each facet to addictive behavior, and; 4) the relative merit of ‘top-down’ versus ‘bottom-up’ approaches to the understanding of impulsivity and the integration of experimental evidence. Each of these issues will be discussed, in turn, with reference to current research into impulsivity and substance abuse. While the proceeding discussion focuses on the UPPS + P model, the issues raised apply equally to any top-down theory of impulsivity driven largely by self-report questionnaire data. It is hoped that this critical review of the literature will stimulate further refinements to the understanding of impulsivity and highlight the importance of theoretical integration across fields.

Section snippets

Factor analysis is not a ‘truth-grinding’ machine

The UPPS and UPPS + P are models of impulsivity borne of factor analysis. Using this statistical technique, Whiteside and Lynam (2001) set out to distil the numerous conceptualizations of impulsivity into core facets common across measures. The Five Factor Model of personality, itself a product of factor analysis, was used as the framework within which to ‘anchor’ these facets within personality more broadly. It should be noted, however, that only three of the Big Five were included as anchors,

Ockham's razor: do additional traits increase explanatory power?

Prior to considering the evidence for additional facets of impulsivity it is worth considering whether a two-factor model provides additional explanatory power over and above a single factor. The relationship between measures tapping into one factor, Reward Sensitivity, and substance use is well-established (e.g., Dissabandara et al., 2014, Franken and Muris, 2006, Gullo and Dawe, 2008, Kabbani and Kambouropoulos, 2013, Kambouropoulos and Staiger, 2004, Knyazev et al., 2004, Loxton and Dawe,

Neurocognitive pathways linking impulsivity facets to addictive behavior

Neurobiological models of addiction vulnerability highlight the importance of two interrelated neural processes: heightened incentive salience arising from the limbic “impulsive” system and impaired response inhibition arising from the prefrontal “executive” system (see Table 1). All drugs of abuse (directly or indirectly) activate the mesolimbic dopamine system, with the nucleus accumbens playing a critical role in their acute reinforcing effects (Koob & Volkow, 2010). Repeated

The relative merit of ‘top-down’ versus ‘bottom-up’ approaches to understanding impulsivity

“In the long run, any account of behaviour which does not agree with the knowledge of the nervous and endocrine system which has been gained through the direct study of physiology must be wrong” — Jeffrey A. Gray (from The psychology of fear and stress [1987; 2nd ed.], p. 241).

It is clear from the above discussion that the lack of theoretical integration with other lines of research is a major obstacle for UPPS + P going forward. While research to-date has failed to support unique contributions

Concluding remarks

In summary, impulsivity is a core vulnerability to addictive behavior. However, five factors are not basic to addiction. There is broad agreement across different levels of analysis that traits related to Reward Sensitivity and Disinhibition play an important and unique role in addictive behavior. These processes are reflected, to varying degrees, in the UPPS + P traits of Sensation Seeking and (Lack of) Premeditation. However, it is likely that UPPS + P Sensation Seeking does not fully capture

Role of Funding Source

Dr Gullo is supported by a National Health and Medical Research Council (NHMRC) of Australia Early Career Fellowship (APP1036365).

Contributors

All authors contributed to the development of the review, including the first draft and subsequent revisions.

Conflict of interest

No conflict declared.

References (134)

  • S. Dawe et al.

    Reward drive and rash impulsiveness as dimensions of impulsivity: Implications for substance misuse

    Addictive Behaviors

    (2004)
  • S. Dawe et al.

    The role of impulsivity in the development of substance use and eating disorders

    Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews

    (2004)
  • L.O. Dissabandara et al.

    Dependent heroin use and associated risky behaviour: The role of rash impulsiveness and reward sensitivity

    Addictive Behaviors

    (2014)
  • S.T. Egan et al.

    Rash-impulsivity, reward-drive and motivations to use ecstasy

    Personality and Individual Differences

    (2010)
  • K.D. Ersche et al.

    Drug addiction endophenotypes: Impulsive versus sensation-seeking personality traits

    Biological Psychiatry

    (2010)
  • H.J. Eysenck

    Four ways five factors are not basic

    Personality and Individual Differences

    (1992)
  • S. Fischer et al.

    Deliberation affects risk taking beyond sensation seeking

    Personality and Individual Differences

    (2004)
  • S. Fischer et al.

    Binge eating, problem drinking, and pathological gambling: Linking behavior to shared traits and social learning

    Personality and Individual Differences

    (2008)
  • S. Fischer et al.

    The relationship of neuroticism and urgency to negative consequences of alcohol use in women with bulimic symptoms

    Personality and Individual Differences

    (2007)
  • I.H.A. Franken

    Behavioral Approach System (BAS) sensitivity predicts alcohol craving

    Personality and Individual Differences

    (2002)
  • I.H.A. Franken et al.

    BIS/BAS personality characteristics and college students' substance use

    Personality and Individual Differences

    (2006)
  • S.M. George et al.

    A prospective study of personality features predictive of early adolescent alcohol misuse

    Personality and Individual Differences

    (2010)
  • J.A. Gray

    The psychophysiological basis of introversion–extraversion

    Behaviour Research and Therapy

    (1970)
  • M.J. Gullo et al.

    Impulsivity and adolescent substance use: Rashly dismissed as “all-bad”?

    Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews

    (2008)
  • M.J. Gullo et al.

    Support for a two-factor model of impulsivity and hazardous substance use in British and Australian young adults

    Journal of Research in Personality

    (2011)
  • T. Hahn et al.

    Neural response to reward anticipation is modulated by Gray's impulsivity

    NeuroImage

    (2009)
  • P.H. Harnett et al.

    Personality, cognition and hazardous drinking: Support for the 2-Component Approach to Reinforcing Substances Model

    Addictive Behaviors

    (2013)
  • R.Y. Kabbani et al.

    Positive expectancies and perceived impaired control mediate the influence of reward drive and rash impulsiveness on alcohol use

    Personality and Individual Differences

    (2013)
  • G. Knyazev et al.

    Personality and substance use in Russian youths: The predictive and moderating role of behavioural activation and gender

    Personality and Individual Differences

    (2004)
  • G.F. Koob et al.

    Drug addiction, dysregulation of reward, and allostasis

    Neuropsychopharmacology

    (2001)
  • N.J. Loxton et al.

    Impulsivity in Hong Kong-Chinese club-drug users

    Drug and Alcohol Dependence

    (2008)
  • M. Lyvers et al.

    Rash impulsiveness and reward sensitivity in relation to risky drinking by university students: Potential roles of frontal systems

    Addictive Behaviors

    (2012)
  • J.E. Maddock et al.

    The College Alcohol Problems Scale

    Addictive Behaviors

    (2001)
  • V. Magid et al.

    The UPPS Impulsive Behavior Scale: Factor structure and associations with college drinking

    Personality and Individual Differences

    (2007)
  • D. Markland

    The golden rule is that there are no golden rules: A commentary on Paul Barrett's recommendations for reporting model fit in structural equation modelling

    Personality and Individual Differences

    (2007)
  • A.T. McLellan et al.

    The fifth edition of the Addiction Severity Index

    Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment

    (1992)
  • F.G. Moeller et al.

    The impact of impulsivity on cocaine use and retention in treatment

    Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment

    (2001)
  • L. Moreno-López et al.

    Trait impulsivity and prefrontal gray matter reductions in cocaine dependent individuals

    Drug and Alcohol Dependence

    (2012)
  • J.T. Nigg et al.

    Poor response inhibition as a predictor of problem drinking and illicit drug use in adolescents at risk for alcoholism and other substance use disorders

    Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry

    (2006)
  • S. Padmala et al.

    Interactions between cognition and motivation during response inhibition

    Neuropsychologia

    (2010)
  • Y. Pardo et al.

    Alcohol use as a behavioural sign of disinhibition: Evidence from J. A. Gray's model of personality

    Addictive Behaviors

    (2007)
  • M.N. Potenza

    Biological contributions to addictions in adolescents and adults: Prevention, treatment, and policy implications

    Journal of Adolescent Health

    (2013)
  • M.N. Potenza et al.

    Found in translation: Understanding impulsivity and related constructs through integrative preclinical and clinical research

    Biological Psychiatry

    (2009)
  • T.A. Ridenour et al.

    Neurobehavior disinhibition, parental substance use disorder, neighborhood quality and development of cannabis use disorder in boys

    Drug and Alcohol Dependence

    (2009)
  • E.S. Barratt

    Anxiety and impulsiveness related to psychomotor efficiency

    Perceptual and Motor Skills

    (1959)
  • J.D. Beaver et al.

    Individual differences in reward drive predict neural responses to images of food

    Journal of Neuroscience

    (2006)
  • A. Bechara

    Decision making, impulse control and loss of willpower to resist drugs: A neurocognitive perspective

    Nature Neuroscience

    (2005)
  • H. Bielau et al.

    Dysregulation of GABAergic neurotransmission in mood disorders: A postmortem study

    Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences

    (2007)
  • J. Block

    A contrarian view of the five-factor approach to personality description

    Psychological Bulletin

    (1995)
  • S.M. Brown et al.

    Neural basis of individual differences in impulsivity: Contributions of corticolimbic circuits for behavioral arousal and control

    Emotion

    (2006)
  • Cited by (125)

    • Genome-wide association studies (GWAS) and post-GWAS analyses of impulsivity: A systematic review

      2024, Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry
    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text