Surgery in the absence of pathology The relationship of patients' presentation to gynecologists' decisions for hysterectomy

https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-3999(00)00103-3Get rights and content

Abstract

Objective: To test predictions from a theory about patients' influence over doctors' treatment decisions when physical symptoms are presented in the absence of physical pathology. Method: We audiotaped 88 gynecological consultations of consecutive patients who presented menstrual symptoms without pathology. Each consultation was coded according to a scheme, developed from previous qualitative research, which identified specific strategies of patients and gynecologists. The occurrence of each strategy was compared between consultations in which hysterectomy was decided upon (N=15) and those leading to conservative treatment. Results: Consultations were more likely to lead to hysterectomy if patients deployed specific strategies, including reporting social effects of symptoms, catastrophization, requesting surgery and citing clinical or lay authority in support. Each strategy could account statistically for gynecologists' perceptions that decisions for hysterectomy reflected patients', rather than gynecologists', influence. Conclusion: The findings are consistent with the theory that, in the absence of physical pathology, patients deploy specific strategies that influence gynecologists to offer surgery.

Introduction

It is often assumed that doctors determine the outcome of medical consultations and that patients tend to be passive in the face of doctors' expertise. Nevertheless, some patients have clear expectations about the effectiveness of different interventions [1], [2] and value their own authority over that of doctors [3], [4], [5]. Therefore, it is to be expected that they will often be motivated to seek specific treatments. However, despite detailed evidence about how doctors influence consultation, we lack a corresponding understanding of patients' influence. Reports of ‘patients' pressure’ for prescription or referral [6], [7] are of limited value because they are defined by doctors' subjective feelings.

A type of consultation in which patients' influence might be particularly important occurs where patients seek physical treatment for physical symptoms in the absence of pathology. In this situation, a doctor's knowledge of the symptoms depends entirely on what the patient says about them. Qualitative analyses have identified several strategies whereby such patients might influence their surgeons to operate, including an emphasis on associated psychosocial problems rather than physical symptoms [1], [8].

Quantification is necessary to test the hypotheses that arise from the qualitative findings. However, the existing schemes for quantifying doctor–patient communication cannot be used for this purpose because they emphasize the form that communication takes, for example, distinguishing verbal effect from information exchange, rather than its function in leading to treatment decisions [9], [10], [11], [12], [13].

In the present study, we, therefore, first developed a procedure for quantifying the aspects of consultation that were identified in previous qualitative research. Then, we used this procedure to test the predictions that arose from the theory that, by using certain strategies, patients influence surgeons to operate in the absence of pathology. For several reasons, we have studied decisions for hysterectomy. It is a relatively common surgical procedure, and is frequently performed in response to mennorrhagia in the absence of physical or hormonal abnormality [14], [15], [16]. Furthermore, the patients' expectations of the effects of gynecological treatment show that hysterectomy is seen as having unique power to improve menstrual problems, while having no more harmful effects than alternative responses, including endometrial resection and drugs [2]. We first confirmed that decisions specifically for hysterectomy were experienced by gynecologists as predominantly reflecting patients' influence. Next, we tested whether specific patient strategies (and gynecologist responses) were more likely to occur in consultations that led to hysterectomy. Finally, we examined whether these strategies explained gynecologists' perception that decisions for hysterectomy reflected patients' influence.

Section snippets

Patients and consultations

Following the approval of ethical committees, consecutive patients at outpatient gynecology clinics at one of the five study hospitals were approached if they had: (i) been referred by their general practitioner to one of 11 participating gynecologists because of menstrual problems; (ii) received one previous consultation as part of the current referral episode; (iii) undergone one or more investigative procedures, including dilatation and curretage (D and C), hysteroscopy or laparoscopy; (iv)

Results

The patients' mean age was 39 years (range: 24–50); 79 (89%) were white Europeans, 54 (61%) were married or cohabiting, 23 (26%) were nulliparous, 59 (67%) described themselves as employed or self-employed. The mean duration of consultation was 11 min (range 2–32); 54 (61%) were with registrars and the remainder were with consultants. Treatment decisions were: hysterectomy (N=15); endometrial resection (5); pharmacotherapy (28); and no change in treatment or further investigation (40). The

Discussion

We have described a way to quantify the elements of the patients' presentation which, from prior qualitative research in the present sample, were hypothesized to influence the gynecologists' treatment decisions. In addition, the procedure detected different ways in which gynecologists state or explain the absence of pathology. The scheme's validity is derived from the qualitative analysis on which it was based [1], [8]. It meets Inui and Carter's [9] recommendations for consultation coding

Acknowledgements

We are grateful for the enthusiastic cooperation of Mr. B.V. Lewis, Professor A. Singer, Mr. D. Liu, Mr. N. Nicholas, Mr. N.C. Drew, Miss H. Morgan and their colleagues and for the assistance of Dr. D. Echlin with coding of transcripts. Mr. B.V. Lewis and Dr. G. Humphris provided helpful comments on an earlier draft of this manuscript.

References (18)

There are more references available in the full text version of this article.

Cited by (18)

  • A systematic review of surgeon-patient communication: Strengths and opportunities for improvement

    2013, Patient Education and Counseling
    Citation Excerpt :

    Another study evaluated the presence of obstetricians/gynecologists and their patients found deficiencies in information sharing, insensitivity to patients’ social conditions, evasion of direct questions, and use of medical jargon [23]. Two other studies described interactions between 9 gynecologists and 88 patients [24,39]. The authors portrayed the surgeons and patients as opponents who use different strategies to assert authority [24].

  • Risk, worry and cosmesis in decision-making for contralateral risk-reducing mastectomy: Analysis of 60 consecutive cases in a specialist breast unit

    2013, Breast
    Citation Excerpt :

    CRRM therefore focuses questions about the extent to which surgery should be offered as a response to consumer request or as a clinical procedure based on clinicians' consideration of risks and benefits. This ambiguity, and the absence of clear decision guidelines, mean that decisions for surgery can in practice reflect, not clinical need, but patient pressure (Case 1), as shown previously in other surgical studies.33–35 This may help to explain why CRRM is increasing particularly in women with higher education and income, who might be best able to assert their preferences for treatment and who might be more aware of health and screening messages.12

  • Ethical issues in the management of somatoform disorders

    2007, Psychiatry
    Citation Excerpt :

    But this call for support can be felt as controlling by their doctors,28 who rarely respond with empathy but more commonly with those very somatic interventions and investigations,29 which only serve to prove what the patient does not have – which find nothing wrong. By the time patients reach secondary care, their attitudes may have hardened into a determination to prove that there really is something wrong.30 For those who make it to a psychiatric referral, it is perhaps unsurprising that discussion of the potentially psychogenic nature of their symptoms is perceived as a further attempt to undermine the validity of their distress, and will be fiercely resisted.

View all citing articles on Scopus
View full text