Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Correlates of unrealistic risk beliefs in a nationally representative sample

  • Published:
Journal of Behavioral Medicine Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Unrealistically optimistic or pessimistic risk perceptions may be associated with maladaptive health behaviors. This study characterized factors associated with unrealistic optimism (UO) and unrealistic pessimism (UP) about breast cancer. Data from the 2005 National Health Interview Survey were analyzed (N = 14,426 women). After accounting for objective risk status, many (43.8%) women displayed UO, 12.3% displayed UP, 34.5% had accurate risk perceptions (their perceived risk matched their calculated risk), and 9.5% indicated “don’t know/no response.” Multivariate multinomial logistic regression indicated that UO was associated with higher education and never smoking. UP was associated with lower education, lower income, being non-Hispanic Black, having ≥3 comorbidities, current smoking, and being overweight. UO was more likely to emerge in younger and older than in middle-aged individuals. UO and UP are associated with different demographic, health, and behavioral characteristics. Population segments that are already vulnerable to negative health outcomes displayed more UP than less vulnerable populations.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. The Gail model is based on population-level data (Gail et al. 1989) and may not adequately predict breast cancer risk at the individual level (Rockhill 2001; Schonfeld et al. 2010), particularly for African American women (Adams-Campbell et al. 2009). However, the model was used to determine individual participant eligibility for the randomized clinical trials of tamoxifen and raloxifene (Fisher et al. 1998; Vogel et al. 2006), is mentioned in the Federal Drug Administration approval of tamoxifen for primary chemoprevention of breast cancer (Vogel et al. 2006), and is posted on heavily visited cancer risk assessment websites (for a review see Waters et al. 2009). Thus, it is not unreasonable to use the Gail model to obtain objective risk estimates.

  2. We also examined unrealistic optimism on the group level. In accordance with established practice (Weinstein 1987), the comparative perceived risk variable was recoded as: −1 (less likely), 0 (about as likely), and +1 (more likely). The mean comparative risk for the entire sample was −0.24 (SE = 0.01), which was significantly different from zero (P < .0001). This demonstrates that, as a group, women perceived their breast cancer risk as lower than the average woman’s risk (i.e., unrealistically optimistic). Readers who are interested in the correlations between the raw comparative risk perception measure and the outcome variables should contact the corresponding author.

  3. Sensitivity analyses indicate that these findings are reasonably robust whether the RR cutoffs for determining whether participants’ objective risk is average, above average, or below average are based on tertiles, quartiles, or quintiles, even after the sequential Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons is conducted (i.e., P < .003 or P < .004 for unrealistic optimism and unrealistic pessimism, respectively). Analyses based on tertiles differed slightly from the quartile analyses. In particular, age2 had an inverted U-shaped relationship for unrealistic pessimism (OR = 0.99, 95% CI [0.98, 1.00], P = .0003), such that younger and older women were less pessimistic than women in their middle years. Analyses based on quintiles also differed slightly. In contrast to the reported (quartile) analysis, people without a high school degree were not less likely to be unrealistically optimistic than people with a degree (OR = 0.88, 95% CI [0.76, 1.02], P = .10), having more than a 4-year degree was not associated with reduced unrealistic pessimism (OR = 0.70, 95% CI [0.50, 0.98], P = .04), and being non-Hispanic Black was not associated with increased unrealistic pessimism (OR = 1.25, 95% CI [1.04, 1.49], P = .015). It should be noted that all of the OR reported in this footnote either fall within or nearly within the confidence intervals reported in Table 3. For instance, the nonsignificant OR of 0.70 for the tertile-based relationship between having more than a 4-year degree and unrealistic pessimism is within 0.01 points of the CI for the same relationship using quartiles (i.e., 0.35–0.69; see Table 3). This suggests that, although the significance levels changed very slightly based upon the objective relative risk cutoffs, the sizes of the relationships—and hence the overall message—are consistent.

  4. Although the paper is focused primarily on unrealistic optimism and unrealistic pessimism across the lifespan, exploratory analyses examined their relationships with appropriate mammogram use. The dataset was restricted to women aged 42–74 without any cancer history, and women were classified according to whether or not they had obtained a mammogram within the previous 2 years (National Cancer Institute 2010). The omnibus F-test indicated that mammography use in the past 2 years was not related to risk perception category overall, P = .81. Readers who wish to obtain additional details about this sub-analysis should contact the corresponding author.

References

  • Adams-Campbell, L. L., Makambi, K. H., Frederick, W. A., Gaskins, M., Dewitty, R. L., & McCaskill-Stevens, W. (2009). Breast cancer risk assessments comparing Gail and CARE models in African-American women. Breast Journal, 15, S72–S75.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ajzen, I. (1985). From intentions to action: A theory of planned behavior. In J. Kuhl & J. Beckman (Eds.), Action control: From cognitions to behaviors (pp. 11–39). New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brain, K., Norman, P., Gray, J., & Mansel, R. (1999). Anxiety and adherence to breast self-examination in women with a family history of breast cancer. Psychosomatic Medicine, 61, 181–187.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Brewer, N. T., Weinstein, N. D., Cuite, C. L., & Herrington, J. E., Jr. (2004). Risk perceptions and their relation to risk behavior. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 27, 125–130.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cameron, L. D. (2003). Anxiety, cognition, and responses to health threats. In L. D. Cameron & H. Leventhal (Eds.), The self-regulation of health and illness behaviour (pp. 157–183). London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dillard, A. J., McCaul, K. D., & Klein, W. M. P. (2006). Unrealistic optimism in smokers: Implications for smoking myth endorsement and self-protective motivation. Journal of Health Communication, 11, 93–102.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dillard, A. J., Midboe, A. M., & Klein, W. M. P. (2009). The dark side of optimism: Unrealistic optimism about problems with alcohol predicts subsequent negative event experiences. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 35, 1540–1550.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Edwards, W. (1954). The theory of decision making. Psychological Bulletin, 51, 380–417.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Facione, N. C. (2002). Perceived risk of breast cancer: Influence of heuristic thinking. Cancer Practice, 10, 256–262.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fagerlin, A., Zikmund-Fisher, B. J., & Ubel, P. A. (2005). How making a risk estimate can change the feel of that risk: Shifting attitudes toward breast cancer risk in a general public survey. Patient Education & Counseling, 57, 249–299.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fishbein, M., & Ajzen, I. (1975). Belief, attitude, intention, and behavior: An introduction to theory and research. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fisher, B., Costantino, J. P., Wickerham, D. L., Redmond, C. K., Kavanah, M., Cronin, W. M., et al. (1998). Tamoxifen for prevention of breast cancer: Report of the National Surgical Adjuvant Breast and Bowel Project P-1 Study. Journal of the National Cancer Institute, 90, 1371–1388.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Freedman, A. N., Graubard, B. I., Rao, S. R., McCaskill-Stevens, W., Ballard-Barbash, R., & Gail, M. H. (2004). How many US women are eligible to use tamoxifen for breast cancer chemoprevention? How many women would benefit? The American Journal of Oncology Review, 3, 47.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gail, M. H., Brinton, L. A., Byar, D. P., Corle, D. K., Green, S. B., Schairer, C., et al. (1989). Projecting individualized probabilities of developing breast cancer for white females who are being examined annually. Journal of the National Cancer Institute, 81, 1879–1886.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Gerend, M. A., Aiken, L. S., West, S. G., & Erchull, M. J. (2004). Beyond medical risk: Investigating the psychological factors underlying women’s perceptions of susceptibility to breast cancer, heart disease, and osteoporosis. Health Psychology, 23, 247–258.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gramling, R., Klein, W. M. P., Roberts, M., Waring, M. E., & Eaton, C. B. (2008). Self-rated cardiovascular risk and 15-year cardiovascular mortality. Annals of Family Medicine, 6, 302–306.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Herzberg, P. Y., Glaesmer, H., & Hoyer, J. (2006). Separating optimism and pessimism: A robust psychometric analysis of the Revised Life Orientation Test (LOT-R). Psychological Assessment, 18, 433–438.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Holm, S. (1979). A simple sequentially rejective multiple test procedure. Scandanavian Journal of Statistics, 6, 65–70.

    Google Scholar 

  • Katapodi, M. C., Dodd, M. J., Facione, N. C., Humphreys, J. C., & Lee, K. A. (2010). Why some women have an optimistic bias about their breast cancer risk. Cancer Nursing, 33, 64–73.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Katapodi, M. C., Dodd, M. J., Lee, K. A., & Facione, N. C. (2009). Underestimation of breast cancer risk: Influence on screening behavior. Oncology Nursing Forum, 36, 306–314.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Katapodi, M. C., Lee, K. A., Facione, N. C., & Dodd, M. J. (2004). Predictors of pereived breast cancer risk and the relation between perceived risk and breast cancer screening: A meta-analytic review. Preventive Medicine, 38, 388–402.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Klein, W. M. P. (1997). Objective standards are not enough: Affective, self-evaluative, and behavioral responses to social comparison information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 763–774.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Klein, W. M. P. (2002). Comparative risk estimates relative to the average peer predict behavioral intentions and concern about absolute risk. Risk, Decision & Policy, 7, 193–202.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kreuter, M. W., & Strecher, V. J. (1995). Changing inaccurate perceptions of health risk: Results from a clinical trial. Health Psychology, 14, 56–63.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Lerman, C. (1995). Effects of individualized breast cancer risk counseling: A randomized trial. Journal of the National Cancer Institute, 87, 286–292.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Lerman, C., & Schwartz, M. (1993). Adherence and psychological adjustment among women at high risk for breast cancer. Breast Cancer Research and Treatment, 28, 145–155.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Lipkus, I. M., Klein, W. M., & Skinner, C. S. (2005). Breast cancer risk perceptions and breast cancer worry: What predicts what? Journal of Risk Research, 8, 439–452.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lipkus, I. M., Kuchibhatla, M., McBride, C. M., Bosworth, H. B., Pollak, K. I., Siegler, I. C., et al. (2000). Relationships among breast cancer perceived absolute risk, comparative risk, and worries. Cancer Epidemiology, Biomarkers and Prevention, 9, 973–975.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Liu, Y., Perez, M., Aft, R. L., Massman, K., Robinson, E., Myles, S., et al. (2010). Accuracy of perceived risk of recurrence among patients with early-stage breast cancer. Cancer Epidemiology, Biomarkers and Prevention, 19, 675–680.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McCaul, K. D., Branstetter, A. D., Schroeder, D. M., & Glasgow, R. E. (1996). What is the relationship between breast cancer risk and mammography screening? A meta-analytic review. Health Psychology, 15, 423–429.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • National Cancer Institute. (2010, Sept 22, 2010). National Cancer Institute FactSheet: Mammograms. Retrieved Nov 9, 2010, from http://www.cancer.gov/cancertopics/factsheet/Detection/mammograms#r1.

  • National Center for Health Statistics. (2006). Data File Documentation, National Health Interview Survey, 2005 (machine readable data file and documentation). Retrieved January 5, 2010, from ftp://ftp.cdc.gov/pub/Health_Statistics/NCHS/Dataset_Documentation/NHIS/2005/srvydesc.pdf.

  • Norem, J. K., & Cantor, N. (1986). Anticipatory and post hoc cushioning strategies: Optimism and defensive pessimism in ‘risky’ situations. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 10, 347–362.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Peters, E. (2008). Numeracy and the perception and communication of risk. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1128, 1–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Radcliffe, N. M., & Klein, W. M. (2002). Dispositional, unrealistic, and comparative optimism: Differential relations with the knowledge and processing of risk information and beliefs about personal risk. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 836–846.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Risen, J. L., & Gilovich, T. (2008). Why people are reluctant to tempt fate. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95, 239–307.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rockhill, B. (2001). The privatization of risk. American Journal of Public Health, 91, 365–368.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Rogers, R. W. (1983). Cognitive and physiological processes in fear appeals and attitude change: A revised theory of protection motivation. In J. T. Cacioppo & R. E. Petty (Eds.), Social psychophysiology: A source book (pp. 153–176). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rose, J. P., Endo, Y., Windschitl, P. D., & Suls, J. (2008). Cultural differences in unrealistic optimism and pessimism: The role of egocentrism and direct versus indirect comparison measures. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34, 1236–1248.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenstock, I. M. (1990). The health belief model. In K. Glanz, F. Lewis, & B. Rimer (Eds.), Health behavior and health education: Theory, research and practice (pp. 39–62). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schonfeld, S. J., Pee, D., Greenlee, R. T., Hartge, P., Lacey, J. V. J., Park, Y., et al. (2010). Effect of changing breast cancer incidence rates on the calibration of the Gail model. Journal of Clinical Oncology, 28, 2411–2417.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Senay, I., & Kaphingst, K. A. (2009). Anchoring-and-adjustment bias in communicating disease risk. Medical Decision Making, 29, 193–201.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Smerecnik, C. M. R., Mesters, I., Verweij, E., de Vries, N. K., & de Vries, H. (2009). A systematic review of the impact of genetic counseling on risk perception accuracy. Journal of Genetic Counseling (Epub ahead of print).

  • Strecher, V. J., Kreuter, M. W., & Kobrin, S. C. (1995). Do cigarette smokers have unrealistic perceptions of their heart attack, cancer, and stroke risks? Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 18, 45–54.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • van der Velde, F. W., van der Pligt, J., & Hooykaas, C. (1994). Perceiving AIDS-related risk: Accuracy as a function of differences in actual risk. Health Psychology, 13, 25–33.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vogel, V. G., Costantino, J. P., Wickerham, D. L., Cronin, W. M., Cecchini, R. S., Atkins, J. N., et al. (2006). Effects of tamoxifen vs raloxifene on the risk of developing invasive breast cancer and other disease outcomes: The NSABP study of tamoxifen and raloxifene (STAR) P-2 trial. JAMA, 295, 2727–2841.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Waters, E. A., Sullivan, H. W., Nelson, W., & Hesse, B. W. (2009). What is my cancer risk? Identifying how Internet-based cancer risk calculators convey individualized risk estimates to the public. Journal of Medical Internet Research, 11, e33.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Weinstein, N. D. (1980). Unrealistic optimism about future life events. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 806–820.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weinstein, N. D. (1987). Unrealistic optimism about susceptibility to health problems: Conclusions from a community wide sample. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 10(5), 481–500.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Weinstein, N. D. (2007). Misleading tests of health behavior theories. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 33, 1–10.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Weinstein, N. D., & Klein, W. M. (1995). Resistance of personal risk perceptions to debiasing interventions. Health Psychology, 14, 132–140.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Witte, K. (1998). Fear as motivator, fear as inhibitor: Using the extended parallel process model to explain fear appeal successes and failures. In P. Andersen & L. K. Guerrero (Eds.), Handbook of communication and emotion: Research, theory, applications, and contexts (pp. 423–450). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This research was supported in part by the National Cancer Institute’s Cancer Prevention Fellowship Program, Center for Cancer Training, Bethesda, MD.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Erika A. Waters.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Waters, E.A., Klein, W.M.P., Moser, R.P. et al. Correlates of unrealistic risk beliefs in a nationally representative sample. J Behav Med 34, 225–235 (2011). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10865-010-9303-7

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10865-010-9303-7

Keywords

Navigation