Skip to main content
Log in

Using Spacing to Enhance Diverse Forms of Learning: Review of Recent Research and Implications for Instruction

  • Review Article
  • Published:
Educational Psychology Review Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Every day, students and instructors are faced with the decision of when to study information. The timing of study, and how it affects memory retention, has been explored for many years in research on human learning. This research has shown that performance on final tests of learning is improved if multiple study sessions are separated—i.e., “spaced” apart—in time rather than massed in immediate succession. In this article, we review research findings of the types of learning that benefit from spaced study, demonstrations of these benefits in educational settings, and recent research on the time intervals during which spaced study should occur in order to maximize memory retention. We conclude with a list of recommendations on how spacing might be incorporated into everyday instruction.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Bahrick, H. P. (1979). Maintenance of knowledge: questions about memory we forget to ask. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 108, 296–308.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bahrick, H. P., & Phelps, E. (1987). Retention of Spanish vocabulary over eight years. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 13, 344–349.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bahrick, H. P., Bahrick, L. E., Bahrick, A. S., & Bahrick, P. E. (1993). Maintenance of foreign language vocabulary and the spacing effect. Psychological Science, 4, 316–321.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Balota, D. A., Duchek, J. M., & Logan, J. M. (2007). Is expanded retrieval practice a superior form of spaced retrieval? A critical review of the extent literature. In J. S. Nairne (Ed.), The foundations of remembering: essays in honor of Henry L. Roediger III (pp. 83–105). London: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berger, S. A., Hall, L. K., & Bahrick, H. P. (2008). Stabilizing access to marginal and submarginal knowledge. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 5, 438–447.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bird, S. (2010). Effects of distributed practice on the acquisition of second language English syntax. Applied PsychoLinguistics, 31, 635–650.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bjork, R. A. (1979). Information-processing analysis of college teaching. Educational Psychologist, 14, 15–23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bloom, K. C., & Shuell, T. J. (1981). Effects of massed and distributed practice on the learning and retention of second-language vocabulary. Journal of Educational Research, 74, 245–248.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carpenter, S. K., & DeLosh, E. L. (2005). Application of the testing and spacing effects to name-learning. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 19, 619–636.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carpenter, S. K., Pashler, H., Wixted, J. T., & Vul, E. (2008). The effects of tests on learning and forgetting. Memory and Cognition, 36, 438–448.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carpenter, S. K., Pashler, H., & Cepeda, N. J. (2009). Using tests to enhance 8th grade students’ retention of US history facts. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 23, 760–771.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cepeda, N. J., Pashler, H., Vul, E., Wixted, J. T., & Rohrer, D. (2006). Distributed practice in verbal recall tasks: a review and quantitative synthesis. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 354–380.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cepeda, N. J., Vul, E., Rohrer, D., Wixted, J. T., & Pashler, H. (2008). Spacing effects in learning: a temporal ridgeline of optimal retention. Psychological Science, 19, 1095–1102.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cepeda, N. J., Coburn, N., Rohrer, D., Wixted, J. T., Mozer, M. C., & Pashler, H. (2009). Optimizing distributed practice: theoretical analysis and practical implications. Experimental Psychology, 56, 236–246.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cull, W. L. (2000). Untangling the benefits of multiple study opportunities and repeated testing for cued recall. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 14, 215–235.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cull, W. L., Shaughnessy, J. J., & Zechmeister, E. B. (1996). Expanding the understanding of the expanding-pattern-of-retrieval mnemonic: toward confidence in applicability. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 2, 365–378.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dempster, F. N. (1987). Time and the production of classroom learning: discerning implications from basic research. Educational Psychologist, 22, 1–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dempster, F. N. (1988). The spacing effect: a case study in the failure to apply the results of psychological research. American Psychologist, 43, 627–634.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dempster, F. N. (1989). Spacing effects and their implications for theory and practice. Educational Psychology Review, 1, 309–330.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dillon, S. (2008). Survey finds teenagers ignorant on basic history and literature questions. The New York Times, A16.

  • Donovan, J. J., & Radosevich, D. J. (1999). A meta-analytic review of the distribution of practice effect: now you see it, now you don't. Journal of Applied Psychology, 84, 795–805.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ebbinghaus, H. (1885/1913). Memory (H. A. Ruger & C. E. Bussenius, Transl.). New York: Teachers College, Columbia University.

  • Fishman, E. J., Keller, L., & Atkinson, R. C. (1968). Massed versus distributed practice in computerized spelling. Journal of Educational Psychology, 59, 290–296.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Glenberg, A. M. (1976). Monotonic and nonmonotonic lag effects in paired-associate and recognition memory paradigms. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 15, 1–16.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Halpern, D. F. (2008, March). 25 learning principles to guide pedagogy and the design of learning environments. Paper distributed at the keynote address at the Bowling Green State University Teaching and Learning Fair. Bowling Green, OH

  • Hintzman, D. L. (1969). Recognition time: effects of recency, frequency, and the spacing of repetitions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 79, 192–194.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hintzman, D. L. (1974). Theoretical implications of the spacing effect. In R. L. Solso (Ed.), Theories in cognitive psychology: The Loyola symposium (pp. 77–97). Potomac: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jones, H. E. (1923). Experimental studies of college teaching. Archives of Psychology, 68, 1–71.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kahana, M. J., & Howard, M. W. (2005). Spacing and lag effects in free recall of pure lists. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12, 159–164.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Karpicke, J. D., & Roediger, H. L., III (2007). Expanding retrieval practice promotes short-term retention, but equally spaced retrieval enhances long-term retention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 33, 704–719.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Landauer, T. K., & Bjork, R. A. (1978). Optimum rehearsal patterns and name learning. In M. M. Gruneberg, P. E. Morris, & R. N. Sykes (Eds.), Practical aspects of memory (pp. 625–632). London: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Logan, J. M., & Balota, D. A. (2008). Expanded vs. equal interval spaced retrieval practice: exploring different schedules of spacing and retention interval in younger and older adults. Aging, Neuropsychology, & Cognition, 15, 257–280.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Melton, A. W. (1970). The situation with respect to the spacing of repetitions and memory. Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior, 9, 596–606.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Metcalfe, J., Kornell, N., & Son, L. K. (2007). A cognitive-science based programme to enhance study efficacy in a high and low risk setting. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 19, 743–768.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moulton, C.-A. E., Dubrowski, A., MacRae, H., Graham, B., Grober, E., & Reznick, R. (2006). Teaching surgical skills: what kind of practice makes perfect? A randomized, controlled trial. Annals of Surgery, 244, 400–409.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, T. O. (1985). Ebbinghaus’s contribution to the measurement of retention: savings during relearning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 11, 472–479.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pashler, H., Bain, P., Bottge, B., Graesser, A., Koedinger, K., McDaniel, M., & Metcalfe, J. (2007). Organizing Instruction and Study to Improve Student Learning (NCER 2007-2004). Washington, DC: National Center for Education Research, Institute of Education Sciences, U. S. Department of Education. Retrieved from http://ncer.ed.gov

  • Pashler, H., Rohrer, D., Cepeda, N. J., & Carpenter, S. K. (2007). Enhancing learning and retarding forgetting: choices and consequences. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14, 187–193.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pyc, M. A., & Rawson, K. (2007). Examining the efficiency of schedules of distributed retrieval practice. Memory and Cognition, 35, 1917–1927.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rea, C. P., & Modigliani, V. (1985). The effect of expanded versus massed practice on the retention of multiplication facts and spelling lists. Human Learning: Journal of Practical Research & Applications, 4, 11–18.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rea, C. P., & Modigliani, V. (1987). The spacing effect in 4- to 9-year-old children. Memory and Cognition, 15, 436–443.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reynolds, J. H., & Glaser, R. (1964). Effects of repetition and spaced review upon retention of a complex learning task. Journal of Educational Psychology, 55, 297–308.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rohrer, D. (2009). The effects of spacing and mixing practice problems. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 40, 4–17.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rohrer, D., & Pashler, H. (2010). Recent research on human learning challenges conventional instructional strategies. Educational Researcher, 39, 406–412.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rohrer, D., & Taylor, K. (2006). The effects of overlearning and distributed practice on the retention of mathematics knowledge. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 20, 1209–1224.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rohrer, D., & Taylor, K. (2007). The shuffling of mathematics practice problems boosts learning. Instructional Science, 35, 481–498.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seabrook, R., Brown, G. D. A., & Solity, J. E. (2005). Distributed and massed practice: from laboratory to classroom. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 19, 107–122.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sobel, H. S., Cepeda, N. J., & Kapler, I. V. (2011). Spacing effects in real-world classroom vocabulary learning. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 25, 763–767.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, K., & Rohrer, D. (2010). The effect of interleaving practice. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 24, 837–848.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tiedeman, H. R. (1948). A study in retention of classroom learning. The Journal of Educational Research, 41, 516–531.

    Google Scholar 

  • Toppino, T. C. (1991). The spacing effect in young children’s free recall: support for automatic process explanations. Memory and Cognition, 19, 159–167.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Toppino, T. C., & DeMesquita, M. (1984). Effects of spacing repetitions on children’s memory. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 37, 637–648.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Toppino, T. C., & DiGeorge, W. (1984). The spacing effect in free recall emerges with development. Memory and Cognition, 12, 118–122.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Verkoeijen, P. P. J. L., Rikers, R. M. J. P., & Özsoy, B. (2008). Distributed rereading can hurt the spacing effect in text memory. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 22, 685–695.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Willingham, D. T. (2002). Allocating student study time: massed vs. distributed practice. American Educator, 47, 37–39.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by a collaborative activity award from the James S. McDonnell Foundation, by the Office of Naval Research (grant N00014-10-1-0072), by the Institute of Education Sciences (US Department of Education grant R305B070537 to H. Pashler and grant R305A110517 to D. Rohrer), and by the National Science Foundation (Center Grant SBE-0542013).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Shana K. Carpenter.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Carpenter, S.K., Cepeda, N.J., Rohrer, D. et al. Using Spacing to Enhance Diverse Forms of Learning: Review of Recent Research and Implications for Instruction. Educ Psychol Rev 24, 369–378 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-012-9205-z

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-012-9205-z

Keywords

Navigation