Article Text
Abstract
Objective To determine the feasibility and acceptability of a computerised treatment for social anxiety disorder for adults who stutter including identification of recruitment, retention and completion rates, large cost drivers and selection of most appropriate outcome measure(s) to inform the design of a future definitive trial.
Design Two-group parallel design (treatment vs placebo), double-blinded feasibility study. Participants: 31 adults who stutter.
Intervention Attention training via an online probe detection task in which the stimuli were images of faces displaying neutral and disgusted expressions.
Main outcome measures Psychological measures: Structured Clinical Interview Global Assessment of Functioning score; Liebowitz Social Anxiety Scale; Social Phobia and Anxiety Inventory; State-Trait Anxiety Inventory; Unhelpful Thoughts and Beliefs about Stuttering. Speech fluency: percent syllables stuttered. Economic evaluation: resource use questionnaire; EuroQol three-dimension questionnaire.
Acceptability: Likert Scale questionnaire of experience of trial, acceptability of the intervention and randomisation procedure.
Results Feasibility of recruitment strategy was demonstrated. Participant feedback indicated that the intervention and definitive trial, including randomisation, would be acceptable to adults who stutter. Of the 31 participants who were randomised, 25 provided data at all three data collection points.
Conclusions The feasibility study informed components of the intervention. Modifications to the design are needed before a definitive trial can be undertaken.
Trial registration number I SRCTN55065978; Post-results.
- Mental Health
- Speech Pathology
- Anxiety Disorders
This is an Open Access article distributed in accordance with the Creative Commons Attribution Non Commercial (CC BY-NC 4.0) license, which permits others to distribute, remix, adapt, build upon this work non-commercially, and license their derivative works on different terms, provided the original work is properly cited and the use is non-commercial. See: http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/4.0/
Statistics from Altmetric.com
Strengths and limitations of this study
The study represents the first attempt to use cognitive bias modification with adults who stutter.
It provides valuable insights into aspects of the design and methodology that will enable us to improve these features in a future trial.
We were unable to gather information that would explain the relatively high attrition rate among those who were randomised.
Introduction
Stuttering is a disorder that affects the fluency of speech. The prevalence of stuttering among adults has been estimated at 0.78%.1 Stuttering is strongly associated with social anxiety disorder, a persistent and excessive fear of being humiliated, scrutinised or negatively evaluated in social situations,2 which may compromise many aspects of life including relationships, education and employment. Up to 60% of adults who are seeking treatment for stuttering experience social anxiety disorder,3 4 compared with 6.8% of the general adult population.5 In the wider population of people who stutter, this figure may be the same, or it could be lower (eg, those with social anxiety disorder may be more likely to seek treatment because of the limitations it places on their lives) or higher (eg, avoidant tendencies associated with social anxiety disorder may make them unwilling to seek treatment).1 There is a lack of adequate provision of speech and language therapy services in the UK6: some areas of the country have no service for adults who stutter,7 and many speech and language therapists have limited formal training in established psychological therapies.8 With increasing pressure on health service resources, an ‘e-mental health’ approach,9 involving online delivery of an intervention that does not require the presence of a trained therapist, could provide a cost-effective means of improving the psychological health of adults who stutter. In addition, e-mental health interventions have the potential to be more convenient for clients, including those who would otherwise have to travel long distances to access the service and those for whom travel is difficult, such as people with disabilities and parents with young children. However, despite the burgeoning availability of online therapies for a wide range of conditions, there is a lack of evidence regarding their effectiveness.10
The primary goal of interventions that address social anxiety in people who stutter must be improvement in mental health status. However, over half of adults who stutter cite improvements in speech fluency as their most important goal when they seek therapy.11 Some people who stutter become so ashamed of their speech disorder that they go to great lengths to disguise it via avoidance techniques; in such cases, the stutter is categorised as ‘covert’, ‘masked’ or ‘interiorized’.12 13 Adults who stutter become more dysfluent in situations that increase their social anxiety,13 14 and recent evidence suggests that the psychosocial dimension of stuttering must be addressed before speech treatment can succeed.15 Given the apparent relationship between anxiety and speech fluency, in the development of interventions for social anxiety in this group, it is desirable to include speech fluency as a secondary outcome measure.
Cognitive behavioural therapy (CBT) is an effective intervention for social anxiety disorder in adults who stutter,4 and recent trials of internet-delivered CBT for adults who stutter4 16–18 have produced promising results. A CBT package tailored specifically to the needs of people who stutter and delivered by a clinical psychologist improved the psychological health of participants,4 and an e-mental health version similarly resulted in improved psychological outcomes and achieved good levels of compliance.16–18 However, CBT does not work for everyone,19 and in its traditional face-to-face mode of delivery, it requires the availability of a suitably trained health professional. Furthermore, despite the negative correlation between social anxiety levels and speech fluency in adults who stutter,13 14 to the authors’ knowledge, there is no evidence that using CBT to alleviate social anxiety also improves speech fluency. It has been speculated that the absence of improvements in fluency after CBT may be because once social anxiety disorder was eliminated, participants no longer feel the need to improve fluency by applying speech restructuring techniques learnt in therapy.4 Alternatively, the controlled and deliberate processing of threat that is one mechanism in CBT20 may reduce the attentional capacity required for applying speech restructuring techniques. Whatever the explanation for the absence of fluency improvements, given these limitations of CBT, is desirable to explore other forms of e-therapy that could be used instead of, or in conjunction with, CBT or other psychological interventions.4 16–18 An intervention approach that relied on automatic, unconscious mechanisms rather than controlled and deliberate processing might allow speech fluency to improve when social anxiety is decreased.
Evidence from cognitive research indicates that anxious and non-anxious individuals deploy attention differently when processing information. For example, in the emotional Stroop task, in which participants have to name the colour in which words are printed as quickly as possible, anxious individuals have longer colour-naming times for words with negative connotations (such as failure) than for neutral words (such as feature).21 This finding is usually interpreted as reflecting interference between the automatic processing of the emotional content and the colour-naming process, though other explanations have been proposed.22 Attentional biases feature heavily in cognitive models of social anxiety.23 Selectively attending to threat causes and maintains feelings of anxiety.24 In particular, socially anxious people have been shown to display attentional biases towards negative facial expressions in the early stages of processing.25 26 A technique that has been used to demonstrate this is called probe detection. For example, in one study that used probe detection,26 participants were shown pairs of faces at very brief exposure times; the two faces might both have a neutral expression, or one face might have a neutral expression and the other an emotional expression (angry or happy). The participants were asked to respond to a visual probe (an upward-pointing or downward-pointing arrow) that appeared in the location previously occupied by one of the faces. A participant who was already attending to an area of the screen where the probe appears should respond relatively quickly to the probe; therefore, relatively short reaction times would indicate a bias towards the stimulus that previously appeared at that location, and relatively long reaction times a bias away from the earlier stimulus. Non-anxious individuals had an attentional bias towards happy faces and away from threatening faces, while those with high levels of social anxiety exhibited the opposite pattern, attending towards threatening faces and away from happy faces.26 There is evidence that non-stuttering anxious people are initially hypervigilant for potential sources of threat, but after about half a second, they then reorient attention away from the threat towards more neutral stimuli.27 28 While initial hypervigilance for threat would increase the individual’s awareness of and anxiety about the threat that they are concerned about, reorientation of attention away from threat stimuli prevents them from exploring and potentially reappraising feared stimuli in a way that could help them to overcome their fear.
Some findings have suggested that people who stutter also have an attentional bias towards negative stimuli during the early stages of processing. On the emotional Stroop task, adults who stutter had longer reaction times to words with negative emotional connotations than non-stuttering controls29 30; in one study,29 adults who stutter had higher levels of both state and trait anxiety than controls, while in the other,30 no measures of anxiety were reported. In a probe detection task, children and adolescents who stutter and who met screening criteria for social anxiety disorder were biased towards looking at schematic faces with sad expressions, unlike non-anxious participants who stutter, who were biased away from such faces.31 However, in another probe detection task, adults who stutter exhibited the same attentional patterns as non-stuttering controls; the stuttering group did not differ from controls on a measure of social anxiety, but had significantly higher levels of trait anxiety.22 Another study found that in a more naturalistic setting where speakers had access to visual feedback from an audience when they were giving a speech, adults who stutter looked less at audience members compared with controls32; the stuttering group scored significantly higher than the controls on a measure of social anxiety. The latter finding may reflect a later stage of processing than the other tasks.
Hypervigilance for social threat can be treated using an approach called attention training,33 34 which retrains processing patterns so that attention is automatically focused away from disapproving facial expressions, with a concomitant improvement in levels of social anxiety.21 35 For example, using the probe detection paradigm, when the letter probe systematically replaced neutral rather than negative faces, thus training individuals to attend away from social threat, there was a significant likelihood that participants would no longer experience social anxiety disorder.34
Although attention training has been successfully used in non-stuttering individuals with social anxiety disorder, there have been no studies to date investigating the use of attention training to address social anxiety in adults who stutter. It is unknown how this intervention would be received by individuals who stutter and whether they would even be interested in taking part in such a study. Recruitment into intervention trials can be difficult for people experiencing mental health problems36 37 and it could be the case that having both a stutter and social anxiety may make participation even more difficult. The Medical Research Council' Complex Interventions Framework (https://www.mrc.ac.uk/documents/pdf/complex-interventions-guidance/) suggests the use of feasibility studies in order to refine the study design and to avoid problems around recruitment, retention, acceptability, compliance and other aspects of delivery in the larger study. Therefore, the present feasibility study was conducted to inform the design of a future clinical trial. Specifically, we aimed to: develop and assess recruitment strategies; assess the suitability of outcome measures for a subsequent trial; estimate SD for outcome measures to inform subsequent sample size calculations; estimate the costs and benefits associated with attention training for adults who stutter; and investigate whether both the intervention and a subsequent randomised controlled trial were likely to be acceptable to the target group, including the acceptability of random allocation.
Methods
Design
Two-group parallel design (treatment vs placebo), double-blinded, with three data collection points (baseline, post-treatment (4 weeks), follow-up 2 (4 months)).
Participants and recruitment
As this was a feasibility study, no formal sample size calculation was carried out, but consistent with recommendations for sample sizes in feasibility studies,38 we aimed to recruit a total of 60 participants. We employed a variety of recruitment methods over a 9-month period. The study was publicised on the website and social media of the British Stammering Association www.stammering.org and via their social media, newsletters, open days and self-help groups; posters and flyers were displayed in local supermarkets, libraries, general practitioner (GP) practices, speech and language therapy clinics, pharmacies and Job Centres and at various venues within the University of East Anglia and local colleges; a press release was distributed, resulting in radio and TV interviews; we publicised the trial via the study’s own website and its Twitter account, including a sponsored tweet, and study information was retweeted to over 4 000 000 Twitter users; we conducted a search and mail-out using National Health Service (NHS) records (GP surgeries and speech and language therapy (SLT) clinics) via searches of data sets using Read Codes 1B92, 2B49, E270 and Eu9y5, which refer to stuttering/stammering (see https://digital.nhs.uk/): we publicised the trial in various events for local speech and language therapists. People who were interested in participating were invited to contact the research team and were then sent an information pack if they had not already received this (eg, via the search and mail-out process or by access the study website). The information pack contained a screening questionnaire which they were asked to return to the study team. The screening questionnaire included the Social Phobia Inventory (SPIN),39 a widely used screening instrument for social anxiety disorder, and also gathered demographic information and details about how respondents heard about the study, their preferred means of contact, their level of computer literacy, current involvement in stuttering or social anxiety research and comorbidity and treatments.
Inclusion and exclusion criteria
Inclusion
Participants had to be at least 18 years old and meet the criteria for risk of social anxiety disorder on the SPIN.39 An additional criterion relating to stuttering rate when measured over the telephone was removed when informal feedback suggested that it was a barrier to recruitment.
Exclusion
Potential participants were excluded if they reported having had CBT during the previous 6 months or speech treatment during the previous 12 months, had an intellectual disability, were currently at risk of self-harm or suicide, were currently using benzodiazepines or were unwilling or unable to maintain a stable dose of any extant psychotropic medication for the duration of the trial.
Intervention and placebo conditions
Participants were randomly allocated in blocks of four or six to the two conditions using a function within the trial data management system at the Norwich Clinical Trials Unit. Apart from the research administrator, all other individuals were blinded after assignment.
Intervention
The intervention consisted of an online computer task. This was programmed and managed by Cambridge Brain Sciences (www.cambridgebrainsciences.com) via their CBSTrials.com platform, which enabled the project team to monitor compliance and issue reminders to participants if necessary, using the preferred means of contact indicated by the participant (email, phone, text or letter).
A set of 16 face images (63 mm x 44 mm), representing eight individuals (four men and four women, representing a range of ages), was selected from the FACES database40 (http://faces.mpib-berlin.mpg.de/album/escidoc:57488). For each individual, one image displayed a disgusted expression and the other a neutral expression. The computer task involved presenting pairs of faces simultaneously.
Each 5 min computer session began with a set of practice trials (in the experimental design sense) followed by the intervention, which consisted of 160 trials. The start of a trial was indicated by the appearance of a fixation cross in the centre of the screen, which remained for 500 ms and on which participants were instructed to focus. A pair of faces was then presented for 500 ms. The images within a pair represented a single individual. Each pair comprised either one face displaying a neutral expression and one displaying a disgusted expression (80% of trials), or two faces displaying a neutral expression. The two faces were separated by 5 degrees of visual angle and were displayed centrally on the computer screen on a grey background. The faces then disappeared and a letter E or F appeared in the location previously occupied by one face. Faces, probe letters and probe positions were counterbalanced. Participants were instructed to respond to the letter probe as quickly and as accurately as possible by pressing the appropriate key on their computer keyboard. In the intervention condition, the letter only ever replaced neutral faces, never disgusted ones.
Participants were asked to complete the online computer task twice a week for 4 weeks. They were given an opportunity to practise a similar task in the baseline data collection session, at which they could ask the researcher questions if necessary. They were given the option of carrying out their eight sessions at home or at the Clinical Trials Unit of the University of East Anglia; all opted to do so at home. To ensure that the visual material was displayed in the same way regardless of the computer they were using, participants were asked to indicate the dimensions of their computer screen at the start of each session.
Placebo
The placebo condition was identical to the experimental condition, except that the letter probe was equally likely to replace either expression.
Ethics and informed consent
Ethical approval for the study was granted by the local research ethics committee (National Research Ethics Committee East of England–Cambridge South, ref: 12/EE/0272). All ethical and research governance procedures were in place before the trial began. All participants gave individual written consent to take part in the trial.
Data collection
Data collection took place at three time points: baseline (prior to commencement of the intervention), post-treatment (approximately 1 week after the final computer session) and follow-up (after a further 3 months). Outcome measures were as follows:
Psychological measures
The following measures were collected at all three time points.
Structured Clinical Interview for DSM–IV–TR Axis I Disorders, Research Version, Non-Patient Edition (SCID).41 The full interview was administered at baseline; only the Social Anxiety section of the interview was administered at the other two time points. The Global Assessment of Functioning score is reported here.
Liebowitz Social Anxiety Scale, 42 a 24-item clinician-administered assessment that focuses on performance anxiety in social situations. The assessment yields separate measures for the anxiety experienced by the participant in different situations and the extent to which they avoid those situations.
Social Phobia and Anxiety Inventory (SPAI), 43 a 45-item self-report assessment that considers specific somatic symptoms, cognitions and behaviours related to social anxiety.
State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI), 44 a 40-item self-report assessment that distinguishes between the person’s temporary, situation-related (state) anxiety and their relatively enduring disposition to experience (trait) anxiety.
Unhelpful Thoughts and Beliefs about Stuttering (UTBAS),45 a 66-item self-report measure that asks about unhelpful thoughts and beliefs that are often reported by people who stutter in clinic.
Speech fluency
Speech fluency was measured to enable comparison with prior research4 and to determine whether changes in social anxiety that might emerge as a result of the intervention had an impact on this variable, since prior research had suggested a correlation between social anxiety and speech fluency.13 The percent syllables stuttered speech fluency measure46 was based on an audio recording lasting approximately 2–3 min collected at each of the three time points. The speech sample was elicited by the interviewer following training by the fifth author, a speech and language therapist specialising in disorders of fluency. Participants were asked to talk about a topic of their own choosing or, if they were unable to think of a topic, the interviewer suggested several possibilities, avoiding topics that might cause emotional distress. At all three sessions, the speech sample was collected early in the interview, before the SCID. The calculation of percent syllables stuttered was carried out by the fifth author.
Economic evaluation
To inform the design of a full economic evaluation as part of a future definitive trial, we estimated questionnaire completion rates, sought to identify how and what cost and effect data would be collected and sought to identify large cost drivers, where a complete case analysis was47 performed with regard to the latter.
Costs were calculated from the perspective of the NHS and personal social services.48Healthcare resource use data were elicited through a self-completed questionnaire administered at baseline and at 4-month follow-up. The questionnaire monitored levels of contact with different healthcare professionals (eg, speech and language therapists, counsellors and GP services), medications and hospital visits and admissions. Costs were assigned to each item of resource use using unit costs of health and social care49 and NHS Reference Costs data,50 estimated at 2012/2013 financial year levels (British pounds). The cost of the intervention itself was not included given that the intervention had already been developed as part of this study and was considered to have a negligible ongoing cost.
The EuroQol five-dimension questionnaire (EQ-5D-3L)51 including the visual analogue scale (EQ-VAS) was used to measure health-related quality of life for which responses were sought at baseline, 5 weeks and 4 months. EQ-5D-3L states were converted into utility scores using the York A1 tariff,52 where 0 is equivalent to death and 1 is full health. Within-trial quality-adjusted life year (QALY) scores were subsequently estimated using the total area under the curve approach.53
Recruitment route
To identify the optimum mode of recruitment, randomised participants were asked how they heard about the trial.
Acceptability
A self-completed structured feedback questionnaire using 5-point Likert responses was administered post-treatment to elicit participants' views and experience of participating in the trial (eg, screening, completion of outcome measures), acceptability of the intervention (completion of the computerised task) and randomisation.
After follow-up measures were completed, participants were informed of their allocation and asked whether they thought it was reasonable that they had been allocated to this condition.
Participants had the opportunity to make open-ended comments both post-treatment and at follow-up about their experience of the intervention and participation in the trial.
Analysis of psychological and fluency measures
Although this was a feasibility study, an analysis was carried out to provide an initial estimate of efficacy for each outcome. The between-group difference was expressed as a difference in means together with a 95% CI. In addition, we used Generalised Estimating Equations (GEEs) to estimate potential intervention effects based on the two follow-up time points simultaneously. This approach incorporates the correlation between time points and does not need an assumption of independence. The models included the baseline values of the dependent variable, time point, study group and a time-by-study group interaction term. These models assumed normally distributed outcomes. These analyses were based on the ‘intention-to-treat’ population; a secondary analysis was carried out based on the intervention actually received. The pooled within-group SD for each outcome was calculated, together with 95% CIs, to inform the sample size of future trials.
Results
Participant recruitment and retention
A total of 312 information packs containing screening questionnaires were sent out: 203 were sent to people identified via NHS records, 109 to people requesting packs after having heard about the study through other routes described above. Forty-nine people (16% of those requesting packs) completed the screening questionnaire. Figure 1 shows the Consolidated Standards of Reporting Trials (CONSORT) flowchart detailing recruitment and retention. Of those who did not meet inclusion criteria, 14 did not meet criteria for risk of social anxiety disorder, one person had received treatment for stuttering in the previous 12 months and one was unwilling or unable to maintain a stable dose of any extant psychotropic medication for the duration of the trial. Thirty-one participants were randomised to the trial, and 25 provided information at all three data collection points. Ten participants needed to be sent at least one reminder to carry out a computer session: eight participants received a single reminder, one participant received reminders about two sessions and one participant was sent reminders about seven sessions. Table 1 shows the baseline characteristics of those who were randomised. There were imbalances at baseline (eg, in age), but given the relatively small sample size, these are not unexpected.
Participants were asked how they heard about the trial. In terms of participants randomised, the British Stammering Association was the most effective recruitment route (n=9), followed by posters in supermarkets (n=5), GP surgeries (5) or SLT clinics (2), GP search and mail-out (2) and Twitter (2). Six other methods (study website, local media, newsletter, SLT clinic search and mail-out, university publicity, word of mouth) resulted in a single participant each.
Psychological and fluency outcome measures
Although all participants had scores consistent with risk of social anxiety disorder according to the SPIN, only three met the full diagnostic criteria in the SCID baseline interview. The SCID excludes a diagnosis of social anxiety disorder in cases where the anxiety is attributable to stuttering, but some researchers in the field of stuttering have modified this definition to include such cases.54 55 According to this modified definition, 17 participants (seven in the attention training condition and 10 in the placebo condition) met the criteria for a diagnosis of social anxiety disorder.
Table 2 summarises data from the psychological and speech fluency outcome measures. The crude between-group mean differences are presented at 5-week and 4-month follow-up with 95% CIs. These interval estimates are wide and in all but one case included 0 (ie, the differences were not statistically significant at the 5% level). In the case of the SCID, however, a statistically significant difference was seen at 4 months. The GEE models, however, adjusting for baseline scores, suggested no difference in the SCID. There was a significant difference in the UTBAS between groups but, again, the CI is very wide, making any firm conclusions difficult to make. There were clear differences with respect to the change in scores over time between the two groups for both the SPAI and STAI, as evidenced by the significant interactions between time and study group, evidenced from the GEE model. These results are reported on an intention-to-treat basis, but it should be noted that one participant who was randomised to the treatment condition actually received the placebo. Analysing the outcomes according to intervention received made no material difference to the results.
Table 3 estimates of the pooled within-group SD (with 95% CIs) for each of the outcomes at each follow-period to aid in sample size calculations. No agreed-upon minimally clinically important differences are available for these measures within this target population. These will be required, through consensus expert opinion, to decide an appropriate sample size for a future definitive trial.
Economic evaluation
The feasibility of conducting an economic evaluation was demonstrated by good response rates, with the Health Services Resource Use (HSRU) and EQ-5D-3L completed by ≥73% participants (online supplementary file 1, table 1). Of the 25 participants completing the feasibility trial, 22 had complete HSRU and EQ-5D-3L questionnaire data available for complete case analysis (11 control, 11 intervention).
Supplemental material
Given the small number of cases and the feasibility design, incremental costs and a recommendation on value for money are purposely not reported. Mean costs and QALY scores reported below should be interpreted with caution.
The source and estimated unit cost for items of resource use are displayed in online supplementary file 1, table 2. Overall, mean levels of resource use reported were low (see online supplementary file 1, table 3). Although mean number of prescriptions showed a slight increase at follow-up in both arms, these prescriptions were not deemed related to the condition. Fifteen per cent (27/181) of total NHS healthcare contacts at baseline and 4 months concerned speech and language therapy, counselling or psychiatry and 17% (38/222) of prescriptions over the same period were for antidepressants (based on all available data). Total mean NHS costs per participant (not including the cost of the intervention) over the 4-month duration of follow-up were £105.83 and £346.27 for intervention and control groups, respectively (see online supplementary file 1, table 3). Costs are largely based on participants recruited from a clinical setting (17/22), with numbers recruited from community and clinical settings evenly distributed across both arms of the trial at baseline (table 1). This pattern was repeated when restricted to complete cases for the economic evaluation (n=22) with 17 from a clinical setting (eight intervention; nine control) and 5 from a community setting (three intervention; two control).
In the Anxiety/Depression dimension of the EQ-5D-3L, 47% (14/30) of respondents reported any problem at baseline compared with the overall population norm of 21%.56 Mean baseline, postintervention and follow-up scores for the EQ-5D-3L and EQ-VAS are shown in online supplementary file 1, table 4, along with the QALY estimate. The mean EQ-5D-3L and EQ-VAS scores were similar for intervention and control arms and over all three times points. The QALY was similar in both control and intervention arms (0.283 and 0.280, respectively). It is difficult to conclude that there is any discernible pattern to these scores given the small numbers in the analyses.
Acceptability
Table 4 summarises data from the feedback questionnaire completed post-treatment, when participants had not yet been informed of their allocation. Of the 26 participants who provided data at this time point, all of those who received the placebo (n=12) thought they had received the placebo; 11 (78.6%) of those who received the intervention also believed they had received the placebo; only 3 participants who received the intervention correctly believed that they had done so. At follow-up, they were informed of their allocation to attention training or placebo and were asked whether they thought it was reasonable that they had been allocated to this condition: only one participant (who had received attention training) felt that randomisation was unreasonable.
Participants had an opportunity to make open-ended comments both post-treatment and at follow-up. Participants made positive comments about the organisation and structure of the study, their interaction with the staff, their own perception of the effect of the treatment, the simplicity of the task, the potential value of such an intervention if effective and their enjoyment of participation. Several expressed a wish to know more about the mechanism underpinning the treatment (they received information about this after data collection was completed). One person noted that the print size of one of the questionnaires was very small. Several remarked that they understood the reasons for randomisation.
Suitability of outcome measures
Outcome measures were generally found to be suitable, but some deserve further comment. The face-to-face nature of the SCID interview caused logistical challenges in terms of availability of interviewer and participant and may also have had a negative impact on recruitment; in future, an equivalent online measure might be preferable. The format of the paper version of the SPAI was such that it was very easy to miss items and, as a result, there were 1.6% missing values for this measure; in the future, it would be better to use a different format or to have the researcher check the form while the participant is present. Finally, although participants generally reported that they did not object to having their speech recorded, this measure could be a barrier to recruitment to a future trial, and consideration should be given to using a self-report measure instead.
Discussion
Despite a diverse recruitment strategy implemented over a period of 9 months, we were only able to randomise approximately half of our original target. Furthermore, only around 16% of the screening questionnaires that were sent out were returned. It is unlikely that our recruitment difficulties were due to a low prevalence of social anxiety disorder among adults who stutter: studies with clinical samples indicate a high prevalence,3 4 and studies using community samples report poor psychological health among adults who stutter.57 58 In general, with regard to recruitment and retention, the main lesson from this study is that a future trial is likely to need an extended recruitment period and wide geographical reach, and would require a similarly diverse recruitment strategy which should involve user organisations such as the British Stammering Association. By contrast, NHS search and mail-out may be a less time-effective and cost-effective method with this client group. The difficulties experienced with recruitment are similar to those outlined in other studies recruiting individuals with mental health difficulties.36 37 Mental health studies which have had success in recruiting participants have highlighted the importance of a flexible and individualised approach to recruitment.59
We do not know the reasons for the 20% attrition rate among those who were randomised. It is possible that they lost interest in the project because they were dubious about the likelihood of success: several participants remarked that it was difficult to see how a computer task could help with anxiety, and the majority of participants thought they had been allocated to the placebo regardless of their actual allocation. This possible barrier to recruitment would need to be addressed in any future trial, though in a manner that would not be likely to impact on effectiveness. Other factors such as the age of participants could also be explored.
With some provisos, the outcome measures were suitable and their use in a future study is feasible for the most part. The small print size of the SPAI may have led to some missing data and was commented on by one of the participants, so in a future trial, an alternative format may need to be adopted. Considering that the fears of those with social anxiety disorder centre on social interaction, perhaps it is inevitable that recruitment and retention will be challenging in a study that requires repeated face-to-face interaction with members of the research team. A possible solution could be to minimise the face-to-face element by using online self-completion measures as much as possible. Consideration also needs to be given to whether and how data about speech fluency should be collected, given that dislike of voice recording could be a reason for problems of recruitment and retention. On the other hand, there could be some value in collecting speech fluency data more regularly, for example, immediately after each computer session. If a speech fluency measure is included in a future study, the duration of the speech sample as well as the mode of collection would need to be considered carefully; the short sample collected in the present study was very short and therefore might not be representative of the individual’s speech, but increasing the length of the speech sample would increase participant burden and might have a negative impact on recruitment and retention. Furthermore, levels of social anxiety could be particularly high among people with a ‘covert’, ‘masked’ or ‘interiorized’ stutter12 13 which they keep concealed by adopting avoidance strategies, but a speech fluency measure could be misleading in their case.
We have shown that it is feasible to collect resource use and EQ-5D data from this population group. However, the mean overall costs do not suggest that this population places a particular burden on the NHS; indeed, direct costs are lower than those reported elsewhere for computerised CBT for anxiety and depression in primary care.60 It is also likely that many of the services reported would have been due to health issues not related to stuttering. Similarly, the mean EQ-5D-3L population norm score has been estimated to be 0.86,61 which is again not noticeably different to the scores for the population in this study.
Feedback from participants was generally good. They reported that their interaction with the project team was positive. They found the computer task undemanding. Almost all the participants indicated that they did not view the prospect of randomisation negatively. It is likely that this form of intervention would be acceptable to potential end-users.
Conclusions
Participants in this feasibility study trial felt that the intervention would be valuable and reported few concerns about the trial itself. If a definitive Randomised Controlled Trial is developed, however, careful consideration will need to be given to participant recruitment, mode of data collection (online vs face to face) and inclusion of a speech fluency measure.
Acknowledgments
The authors would like to thank the participants, the British Stammering Association and the South Norfolk Clinical Commissioning Group.
References
Footnotes
Contributors JMcA was the CI; she led on the design of the study and composed the first draft of the paper. SG, AC,KH and MK had the main responsibilities associated with recruitment and data collection. LS, HR, BM, TDT and JH, as well as the authors mentioned earlier, contributed to the design and/or analysis of the study. All authors read and commented on the manuscript and approved the final draft.
Funding This paper presents independent research funded by the National Institute for Health Research (NIHR) under its Research for Patient Benefit (RfPB) Programme (Grant Reference Number PB-PG-0610-22225). The views expressed are those of the author(s) and not necessarily those of the NHS, the NIHR or the Department of Health.
Competing interests None declared.
Ethics approval NRES Committee East of England–Cambridge South.
Provenance and peer review Not commissioned; externally peer reviewed.
Data sharing statement Data from a cognitive bias experiment and a user questionnaire were also gathered. All data are available only to the study team.