Article Text

Original research
Running on empty: a longitudinal global study of psychological well-being among runners during the COVID-19 pandemic
  1. Helene Tilma Vistisen1,2,
  2. Kim Mannemar Sønderskov3,4,
  3. Peter Thisted Dinesen5,6,
  4. René Børge Korsgaard Brund7,
  5. Rasmus Østergaard Nielsen8,9,
  6. Søren Dinesen Østergaard1,2
  1. 1Department of Affective Disorders, Aarhus University Hospital, Aarhus, Denmark
  2. 2Department of Clinical Medicine, Aarhus University, Aarhus, Denmark
  3. 3Department of Political Science, Aarhus University, Aarhus, Denmark
  4. 4Centre for the Experimental-Philosophical Study of Discrimination, Aarhus University, Aarhus, Denmark
  5. 5Department of Political Science, University of Copenhagen, Copenhagen, Denmark
  6. 6Department of Political Science, University College London, London, UK
  7. 7Sport Sciences, Department of Health Science and Technology, Aalborg University, Aalborg, Denmark
  8. 8Department of Public Health, Aarhus University, Aarhus, Denmark
  9. 9Research Unit for General Practice, Aarhus University, Aarhus, Denmark
  1. Correspondence to Dr Søren Dinesen Østergaard; soeoes{at}rm.dk

Abstract

Objectives There are indications that the COVID-19 pandemic has had a profound negative effect on psychological well-being. Here, we investigated this hypothesis using longitudinal data from a large global cohort of runners, providing unprecedented leverage for understanding how the temporal development in the pandemic pressure relates to well-being across countries.

Design Prospective cohort study.

Setting Global.

Participants We used data from the worldwide Garmin-RUNSAFE cohort that recruited runners with a Garmin Connect account, which is used for storing running activities tracked by a Garmin device. A total of 7808 Garmin Connect users from 86 countries participated.

Primary and secondary outcome measures From 1 August 2019 (prepandemic) to 31 December 2020, participants completed surveys every second week that included the five-item WHO Well-Being Index (WHO-5). Pandemic pressure was proxied by the number of COVID-19-related deaths per country, retrieved from the Coronavirus Resource Centre at Johns Hopkins University. Panel data regression including individual- and time-fixed effects was used to study the association between country-level COVID-19-related deaths over the past 14 days and individual-level self-reported well-being over the past 14 days.

Results The 7808 participants completed a total of 125 409 WHO-5 records over the study period. We found a statistically significant inverse relationship between the number of COVID-19-related deaths and the level of psychological well-being—independent of running activity and running injuries (a reduction of 1.42 WHO-5 points per COVID-19-related death per 10 000 individuals, p<0.001).

Conclusions This study suggests that the COVID-19 pandemic has had a negative effect on the psychological well-being of the affected populations, which is concerning from a global mental health perspective.

  • depression & mood disorders
  • COVID-19
  • mental health

Data availability statement

The data used for the present study cannot be shared as the informed consent specifies that they will be stored only at servers at Aarhus University, Denmark.

http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/4.0/

This is an open access article distributed in accordance with the Creative Commons Attribution Non Commercial (CC BY-NC 4.0) license, which permits others to distribute, remix, adapt, build upon this work non-commercially, and license their derivative works on different terms, provided the original work is properly cited, appropriate credit is given, any changes made indicated, and the use is non-commercial. See: http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/4.0/.

Statistics from Altmetric.com

Request Permissions

If you wish to reuse any or all of this article please use the link below which will take you to the Copyright Clearance Center’s RightsLink service. You will be able to get a quick price and instant permission to reuse the content in many different ways.

STRENGTHS AND LIMITATIONS OF THIS STUDY

  • Psychological well-being was tracked every second week over several months prior to and during the COVID-19 pandemic.

  • The study was based on data from 7808 participants representing 86 countries.

  • The participants were self-enrolled runners, who are likely more psychologically robust than the general population.

  • Data on nationwide and regional lockdowns from the 86 countries were not available.

Introduction

Beyond its obvious negative health consequences for those directly infected with coronavirus, the COVID-19 pandemic—and the ensuing public health measures implemented to prevent its spreading (eg, lockdowns and restrictions on social gatherings)—is likely to have had adverse effects on psychological well-being more broadly due to, inter alia, the uncertainty, the disruption of everyday routines, and the social disconnectedness it has induced.1 2

Previous longitudinal studies, tracking the development in psychological well-being over time by means of surveys, have provided initial evidence documenting the negative consequences of the COVID-19 pandemic. While informative, these studies generally suffer from one or more significant drawbacks. First, only a subset of these studies has a prepandemic baseline measurement that is necessary to enable any inferences about the consequences of the pandemic.3–12 Further, even if prepandemic benchmarks are available, they are typically few and dating back a longer period of time (often years) before the onset of the pandemic.4–14 This compromises the value of the prepandemic measure, and, by implication, the credibility of any observed change in well-being after the onset of the pandemic. Several pre-pandemic measurements taken over a period leading directly up to the pandemic, would strengthen the case further for the pandemic causing an observed decline in psychological well-being. Second, beyond the consequences of the COVID-19 pandemic in toto, previous studies—including our own13 15–17—have produced limited knowledge about how psychological well-being covaries with pandemic pressure (ie, the severity of the COVID-19 pandemic) given the absence of systematic post-pandemic measurements of well-being. If psychological well-being changes in tandem with the ebb and tide of the pandemic waves, it strengthens the claim of the pandemic influencing well-being. Third, the existing results are typically from single-country studies.3 9 18 19 While this is a natural starting point, this means that any (inverse) correspondence between pandemic pressure and psychological well-being could be due to other temporal changes that causes changes in well-being (eg, seasonal changes in daylight or weather).16 20 Using data from several countries with variation in pandemic pressure and seasonal conditions can alleviate this concern, and would therefore lend further credibility to the robustness of the negative effect of the pandemic pressure on psychological well-being.

Against the backdrop of previous studies and their shortcomings, the aim of this study was to investigate the dose–response relationship between pandemic pressure (proxied by number of COVID-19-related deaths) and psychological well-being using shortly-spaced individual-level panel survey data from more than 80 countries with extensive measurement points both before and after the inception of the pandemic. The data stems from a large global cohort of runners (the Garmin-RUNSAFE Running Health Study21), and therefore, to fortify our results against idiosyncratic features of this sample, we used auxiliary data on the participants’ running-related characteristics (activities and injuries), to establish that the relationship between the pandemic pressure and psychological well-being is independent of changes in these characteristics and hence likely generalises more broadly.

Methods

Data source

We used data from the international worldwide Garmin-RUNSAFE Running Health Study that recruited English-speaking runners aged 18+ with a Garmin Connect account. Garmin connect is a tool for storing and sharing running activities from a Garmin device.21 Enrolment was open from 1 August 2019 (prepandemic) to 31 December 2020. For further details on the recruitment, see Nielsen et al.21

Data collection

At enrolment, the participants in the Garmin-RUNSAFE Running Health Study provided information on country of residence and date of birth. Furthermore, they gave access to daily information on running distance (in metres) during follow-up (from enrolment to 31 December 2020) from their Garmin Connect account. From the time of enrolment to 31 December 2020, the RUNSAFE participants were asked to complete surveys every 2 weeks (sent via email) that included the five-item WHO Well-Being Index (WHO-5)22—a psychometrically valid and widely used measure of psychological well-being experienced over the past 2 weeks. The five WHO-5 items are: ‘I have felt cheerful and in good spirit’, ‘I have felt calm and relaxed’, ‘I have felt active and vigorous’, ‘I woke up feeling fresh and rested’ and ‘My daily life has been filled with things that interest me’. Each item is scored from 0 (none of the time) to 5 (all the time). The WHO-5 total score is calculated by adding the individual item scores and multiplying by four (ranges from 0 (complete lack of well-being) to 100 (maximum well-being)). The participants also provided weekly information on running-related injuries/problems. Specifically, they were asked to indicate which day in the past week a running-related injury/problem interfered with their running activity and/or affected their activities of daily living.

Patient or public involvement

Patients or the public were not involved in the design, or conduct, or reporting, or dissemination plans of this research.

Study population

For this study, we used data from all participants in the Garmin-RUNSAFE Running Health Study with information on country of residence and with ≥1 completed WHO-5 questionnaire on psychological well-being.

Data on COVID-19-related deaths

The daily number of COVID-19-related deaths per country was retrieved from the Coronavirus Resource Centre at John Hopkins University.23 The few instances (0.19%) of negative daily deaths (due to changing definitions) were replaced by the mean number of deaths from the two neighbouring dates. We opted for using country-specific death rates because it, unlike other measures, presumably is highly comparable within countries over time. Other measures like incidence rates of COVID-19 and transmissibility depends heavily on test rates, which varied substantially within countries over time due to variation in availability of tests, pandemic pressure etc.

Statistical analysis

The data described above were organised in person-week units. Specifically, for each week in the follow-up period (1 August 2019–31 December 2020), we computed participant-level WHO-5 total scores (ie, their well-being the past 14 days), running distance over the past 14 days (in metres), running-related injuries/problems (days affected of the past 14 days), as well as the number of COVID-19-related deaths per 10 000 inhabitants (in the country of the participant) for the past 14 days. The rationale behind the weekly and not 2 weekly organisation was that even though the WHO-5 questionnaires were send out every second week, responses were returned throughout the subsequent 14-day deadline period. If a participant filled in the WHO-5 twice within the same week, the last WHO-5 total score was used.

The following analyses were carried out: First, the cohort was characterised using descriptive statistics. Subsequently, the relationship between country-level COVID-19-related deaths over the past 14 days and the level of psychological well-being over the past 14 days (WHO-5 total score) was assessed via a linear regression model including individual- and time-fixed effects, which reduces the risk of confounding from stable individual- and country-level characteristics as well as general trends in well-being during the study period:

Embedded Image

Here, WHO-5it is the WHO-5 total score for individual i for the time period t (past 14 days), Deathsit is the number of deaths per 10 000 inhabitants in i’s country of residence over the time period t, RunningActivityit is i’s running activity (total metres) over time period t, and injuryit is the number of days over time period t where i’s activity was affected by a running-related injury/problem. The three remaining terms represent unobserved factors affecting the WHO-5 total score: ai is time-invariant and individual-specific, ut is individual-invariant and time-specific, and ϵit represents unobserved determinants of the WHO-5 total score that vary across both individual and time. To remove a, we included a full set of individual-level fixed effects, and to remove ut we included time-fixed effects. Subsequently, we ran the same analysis for each of the five WHO-5 items (replacing WHO-5it in the equation shown above). The rationale behind this analytical model is illustrated in the directed acyclic graph shown in online supplemental figure 1. To check the robustness of the model, we conducted leave-one-out analysis excluding one country from the model at the time. As secondary analyses, to explore potential non-linear effects of the number of COVID-19-related deaths, square root, natural logarithmic and quadratic terms were employed (see online supplemental methods for further description).

Finally, to test whether the RUNSAFE participants had higher psychological well-being than the general population (a priori hypothesis), we compared the WHO-5 total scores of the Danish RUNSAFE participants with the WHO-5 total scores from the first three waves of the COVID-19 Consequences Denmark Panel Survey 2020.13 15 16 The WHO-5 total scores from the COVID-19 Consequences Denmark Panel Survey 2020 respondents were weighted on gender, age, education, region and political party choice in the last election in order to render them representative of the Danish population. Only WHO-5 data from overlapping periods of data collection in the two surveys were included, namely 31 March 2020–6 April 2020; 22 April 2020– 30 April 2020 and 20 November 2020–8 December 2020.13 15 16

All analyses were carried out using Stata V.17.0 (StataCorp) with 0.05 as the threshold for statistical significance.

Results

In the period from 1 August 2019 to 31 December 2020, a total of 7808 RUNSAFE-participants completed the WHO-5 questionnaire at least once. Data from these 7808 participants were included in the analyses (see figure 1). The characteristics of the participants are listed in table 1.

Table 1

Charateristics of the 7808 participants at enrolment

Figure 1

Flow chart of the study population and WHO-5 observations. WHO-5, five-item WHO Well-Being Index.

The participants covered 86 different countries, the age range was 18–88 years, mean age was 47.3 years (SD=10.61) and 76% were men. The maximum follow-up was 17 months including 39 biweekly WHO-5 questionnaires, and 75 weekly injury questionnaires. The total number of completed WHO-5 questionnaires was 125 409 and the median number of completed WHO-5 questionnaires among the 7808 participants was 12 (IQR: 3–31). A total of 980 (12.6%) of the participants had completed the WHO-5 only once and thereby only contributed to the estimation of the country- and the time fixed effects. For an illustration of the distribution of participants and completed WHO-5 questionnaires across countries, see online supplemental figure 2.

Among the 7808 respondents, 7175 (91.9%) had tracked their running activity through Garmin Connect at least once (with a total of 230 169 weeks with information on running activity), and 7759 (99.4%) had filled out the weekly questionnaire about running-related injuries at least once (with a total of 257 171 weeks with information on injuries). For an illustration of the tracking of running activity and completed injury questionnaires over the course of the study, see online supplemental figure 3.

The range in number of COVID-19-related deaths per 10 000 (within a country) during a fourteen-day period was 0 to 3.65 with a median of 0.02 (IQR: 0.00–0.35) in the study period, and a median of 0.31 (IQR: 0.04–0.59) in the period from March 2020 to December 2020. For an illustration of the number of COVID-19-related deaths, the number of study participants, and the level of psychological well-being of these participants over the study period (see figure 2).

Figure 2

Number of participants (orange bars), COVID-19 deaths (grey bars) and mean WHO-5 total score (red line) over the course of the study. The line representing the mean WHO-5 total score is generated using a lowess smoother. The orange bars represent the number of participants having completed the WHO-5 at least once in the specific month. WHO-5, five-item WHO Well-Being Index.

The linear association between the number of COVID-19-related deaths per 10 000 and psychological well-being (WHO-5 total score) is illustrated in figure 3 and reported in table 2.

Table 2

Individual fixed-effects linear-regression analyses with time fixed effects (crude* and adjusted† model)

Figure 3

The association between COVID-19-related deaths per 10 000 and psychological well-being (WHO-5 total score). WHO-5, five-item WHO Well-Being Index.

The results show a statistically significant inverse relationship (regression coefficient of −1.42, 95% CI −2.16 to −0.67), which remained when excluding running activity and running related injuries/problems from the model (table 2) and when leaving specific countries out of the analysis one at the time (online supplemental table 1). The number of COVID-19-related deaths was also inversely associated with the five individual WHO-5 items (table 2). The results of the three non-linear analyses were also consistent with an inverse relationship between the number of COVID-19-related deaths per 10 000 and psychological well-being (table 2 and online supplemental table 2). Specifically, all analyses showed that the strength of the inverse relationship decreased at higher levels of COVID-19-related deaths (see online supplemental figure 4). The results of the quadratic model indicated that the relationship could be positive at very high levels of COVID-19 related deaths (approximately ≥2.0 COVID-19-related deaths per 10 000 inhabitants). This specific finding is, however, uncertain, because of few observations with very high levels of COVID-19-related deaths (out of the 125 409 person-week observations, only 1974 (1.6%) had a rate ≥2.0 deaths per 10 000 inhabitants).

Finally, and as expected, the psychological well-being of the participants in the Garmin-RUNSAFE Running Health Study (mean WHO-5 total score of 71.6, 95% CI 70.0 to 73.2) was substantially higher than that of the participants from the COVID-19 Consequences Denmark Panel Survey 2020 (mean WHO-5 total score of 63.2, 95% CI 62.7 to 63.7), when compared across the same time periods.

Discussion

In this longitudinal study of 7808 runners from 86 countries, we found a statistically significant inverse relationship between the number of COVID-19-related deaths and the level of psychological well-being, which was independent of running activity and running injuries. These results were generally robust across models and sensitivity (leave-one-out) analyses.

To our knowledge, this study is the first to have tracked the psychological well-being of individuals from >80 countries over several months prior to and during the COVID-19 pandemic. The results bolsters and furthers findings from studies using less fine-grained data and less rigorous designs in showing that there is a dynamic inverse relationship between the pandemic pressure and the level of psychological well-being.3–14 They are also in line with studies having focused on the opposite of psychological well-being during the COVID-19 pandemic, namely symptoms of anxiety and depression, where a positive relationship with the pandemic presure has been the most consistent finding.24–28 Irrespective of the definition of outcome, this body of litterature clearly suggests that the COVID-19 pandemic is not only a global crisis from a physical health perspective, but also from a mental health/psychological perspective, as acknowledged by the WHO.29

Although this study has strengths, in particular due to the availability of fine-grained prepandemic and in-pandemic data on psychological well-being from many countries across continents, there are also important limitations to take into account. First, participants in the survey are self-enrolled and the sample is therefore probably not representative of runners from the included countries, and—given the heterogeneous participation patterns across countries (table 1; online supplemental figure 2)—certainly not representaitve of the global population of runners. Second, participation varies over time and there are clear signs of panel attrition over the study period, which also raises questions about generalisability. The inclusion of individual fixed effects, and by implication, country fixed effects, alleviates some of this concern, as it removes the influence of individual-level and country-level variables. Nevertheless, generalisability of the results beyond the specific participants is uncertain. Third, and relatedly, the fact that all participants are runners is also suboptimal with regard to the generalisability of the results.We also notice that the sample is predominantly male (76%), which is likely due to the recruitment method via Garmin Connect—a platform that may be more appealing to male than female runners. Runners are known to be healthier than the general population—both physically and psychologically30–33—as also demonstrated by the comparison of psychological well-being between the participants in the Garmin-RUNSAFE Running Health Study and the participants from the COVID-19 Consequences Denmark Panel Survey 2020. However, while runners are not representative of the general population, the fact that they are considered to be quite robust from a psychological perspective, implies that the inverse relationship is likely to be stronger in the general population, thereby rendering our estimate a conservative one. Fourth, with regard to the exposure, namely the number of COVID-19-related deaths, there are inter-country differences in the reporting/operationalisation.34 35 This does not constitute a major problem, because country differences are removed with the individual-fixed effects. Nevertheless, identical reporting practices would have been preferable. Fifth and relatedly, data on nationwide and regional lockdowns from the 86 countries were not available to us. We were therefore unable to investigate whether the observed negative relationship between COVID-19-related deaths and psychological well-being is driven by the lockdowns—a downstream consequence of pandemic pressure—as has been suggested by some, but not all, other studies.36 37 Sixth, although the results of this study do not suggest that running activity and running related injuries/problems have marked effects on the impact of the COVID-19 pandemic pressure on psychological well-being, controlled intervention studies are required to clarify the question of causality. Such studies are, however, also associated with challenges—in particular due to the difficulties with regard to blinding, which is virtually impossible. Seventh, our data does not cover the period from 1 January 2021 and onwards, but based on other studies covering this period, it seems that the psychological well-being of people has kept covarying with the pandemic pressure.17 36 Given that the pandemic pressure is relatively low at the time of writing, it seems reasonable to assume that its negative influence on psychological well-being is correspondingly low.

In conclusion, based on analysis of longitudinal data from 7808 runners from 86 countries, this study substantiates the notion that the COVID-19 pandemic has had a negative impact on the psychological well-being of the affected populations. As the COVID-19 pandemic is ongoing and may develop further due to occurrence of new viral variants, these findings are concerning from a global mental health perspective.

Data availability statement

The data used for the present study cannot be shared as the informed consent specifies that they will be stored only at servers at Aarhus University, Denmark.

Ethics statements

Patient consent for publication

Ethics approval

This study involves human participants but all participants in the Garmin-RUNSAFE Running Health Study completed an online informed consent form prior to enrolment. As this was an observational study, the local ethics committee in the Central Denmark Region waived registration (Request number: 227/2016- Record number: 1-10-72-189-16) in accordance with the Danish Act on Research Ethics Review of Health Research Projects, Section 14, no. 2. The Danish Data Protection Agency approved the study (the Danish Data Protection Agency’s record number: 2015-57-0002; Aarhus University’s record number: 62908, serial number 309), including the data collection procedure and data storage.

Acknowledgments

The authors are grateful to Garmin International, Olathe, USA for assisting with recruitment of study participants and facilitating interpretation of running metrics from Garmin Connect.

References

Supplementary materials

  • Supplementary Data

    This web only file has been produced by the BMJ Publishing Group from an electronic file supplied by the author(s) and has not been edited for content.

Footnotes

  • Contributors The study was designed in collaboration between all authors. The analyses were carried out by HTV and KMS. The results were interpreted by all authors. HTV, PTD, KMS and SDØ wrote the first draft of the manuscript, which was subsequently revised for important intellectual content by RBKB and RØN. All authors approved the final version of the manuscript prior to submission. HTV is the guarantor.

  • Funding The study is supported by unconditional grants from the Novo Nordisk Foundation to SDØ (Grant number: NNF20SA0062874) and from Aarhus University Research Foundation (grant number: AUFF-E-2015-FLS-9-9) and the Danish Rheumatism Association to RØN (grant number: R160-A5157). SDØ is further supported by grants from the Lundbeck Foundation (grant numbers: R358-2020-2341 and R344-2020-1073), the Danish Cancer Society (grant number: R283-A16461), the Central Denmark Region Fund for Strengthening of Health Science (grant number: 1-36-72-4-20), the Danish Agency for Digitisation Investment Fund for New Technologies (grant number 2020-6720), Independent Research Fund Denmark (grant number: 7016-00048B).

  • Disclaimer These funders had no role in the study design; in the collection, analysis, and interpretation of data; in the writing of the report; and in the decision to submit the paper for publication.

  • Competing interests SDØ received the 2020 Lundbeck Foundation Young Investigator Prize. Furthermore, SDØ owns units of mutual funds with stock tickers DKIGI and WEKAFKI, as well as units of exchange traded funds with stock tickers TRET and EUNL. The remaining authors report no conflicts of interest.

  • Patient and public involvement Patients and/or the public were not involved in the design, or conduct, or reporting, or dissemination plans of this research.

  • Provenance and peer review Not commissioned; externally peer reviewed.

  • Supplemental material This content has been supplied by the author(s). It has not been vetted by BMJ Publishing Group Limited (BMJ) and may not have been peer-reviewed. Any opinions or recommendations discussed are solely those of the author(s) and are not endorsed by BMJ. BMJ disclaims all liability and responsibility arising from any reliance placed on the content. Where the content includes any translated material, BMJ does not warrant the accuracy and reliability of the translations (including but not limited to local regulations, clinical guidelines, terminology, drug names and drug dosages), and is not responsible for any error and/or omissions arising from translation and adaptation or otherwise.