Article Text
Abstract
Introduction Checking behaviour (CB) occurs in various mental health problems. Cognitive-behavioural models for these mental disorders share similar theoretical assumptions. Thus, they postulate a negative reinforcing effect of CB by reducing negative affect (ie, anxiety) and a maintenance of the pathology due to a lack of reality testing of concerns. This paper details methods for a systematic review that will be conducted to synthesise empirical evidence testing these theoretical assumptions across obsessive–compulsive, generalised anxiety, eating, body dysmorphic and illness anxiety disorder. The results are expected to foster our understanding of the mechanisms of action underlying CB, which is of high clinical relevance. Depending on whether or not the findings confirm the model assumptions regarding CB, the focus of treatments would need to be intensified or modified.
Methods and analysis We will search PsycINFO, PubMed, PSYNDEX and Scopus for studies investigating the emotional state in which CB is being used as well as the immediate and longer-term effects of CB on cognitive and emotional measures in clinical and analogue samples. The selection process, data extraction and quality assessment of included studies will be performed by two independent reviewers. In the case of inconsistencies, a third reviewer will be involved. Study results will be reported in a narrative synthesis.
Ethics and dissemination Ethics approval will not be required as this is a protocol for systematic review. The results are mainly disseminated through peer-reviewed publications.
PROSPERO registration number CRD42021238835.
- Eating disorders
- Anxiety disorders
- Adult psychiatry
- MENTAL HEALTH
This is an open access article distributed in accordance with the Creative Commons Attribution Non Commercial (CC BY-NC 4.0) license, which permits others to distribute, remix, adapt, build upon this work non-commercially, and license their derivative works on different terms, provided the original work is properly cited, appropriate credit is given, any changes made indicated, and the use is non-commercial. See: http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/4.0/.
Statistics from Altmetric.com
Strengths and limitations of this study
The protocol is written following the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis Protocols guidelines.
As the review includes non-randomised studies that are likely to produce evidence of low certainty, risk of bias and the strength of evidence collected from each study will be assessed using the Grading of Recommendations Assessment, Development and Evaluation system.
The heterogeneity and the expected small number of studies represent a limitation of this systematic review.
Introduction
Rationale
Safety behaviour represents a core feature of various mental disorders1 2 and is defined as ‘actions taken to prevent, avoid or escape a feared outcome’.3 In the narrower sense, this includes behaviours such as taking sedatives, not going to certain places without another person or always carrying a bottle of water. Furthermore, it comprises avoidance behaviours and checking behaviour (CB), which manifests in different ways depending on the respective disorder. The earliest descriptions of the latter can be found regarding obsessive–compulsive disorders (OCD).4 5
In OCD, CB is the most common compulsion6 and manifests, for example, as controlling the absence of potential sources of danger in one’s surroundings (eg, stove turned off to prevent fire, windows or doors locked to prevent burglary) or repetitive requests for reassurance from others.7 8 Closely related to this, CB in generalised anxiety disorder (GAD) is also described,9–11 but mainly in terms of interpersonal checking (ie, seeking reassurance from others, eg, before making decisions, when engaging in activities or asking a loved one if he or she is upset).8 12 While in OCD and GAD, checking primarily refers to objects, environment, relationships and achievement, in other disorders, the main focus of CB is one’s own body. Disorders with body-related CB include eating disorders (EDs),13 body dysmorphic disorder (BDD)14 and illness anxiety disorder (IAD).15 In EDs, that is, anorexia nervosa, bulimia nervosa and binge-eating disorder, CB expresses itself as inspecting one’s own body in terms of its weight or shape, and manifests in behaviours such as repeated weighing, measuring the circumference of body parts, inspecting one’s body or individual body parts in the mirror, seeking reassurance about one’s appearance and comparing it to others.13 16 CB in BDD is described as inspecting one’s perceived defect by looking at it in the mirror or other reflective surfaces (eg, shop windows, car mirrors) in a ritualistic way, taking photos, comparing it with other people (in real life, media, photos of oneself in the past), checking its size or contour by touching it with one’s fingers, and asking others for reassurance (eg, whether the perceived flaw has become worse or is adequately camouflaged).17–19 Whereas, checking in EDs and BDD refers to figure, weight or appearance, checking in IAD focuses on health. It manifests as repeatedly inspecting one’s body for signs of illness (eg, breast self-examination, lymph node palpation) or seeking reassurance from others about health and signs of a severe illness (eg, family, friends, healthcare physicians or alternative sources such as medical textbooks).15 20
Although CB refers to different domains (eg, weight, illness, safety of the environment) in all five disorders, cognitive-behavioural theories regarding the mechanism of action of checking are very alike7 21–24. Three central theoretical postulates are formulated in each case. First, it is proposed that CB is used primarily in order to gain relief from unpleasant emotional states.22 25 Relatedly, the second theoretical postulate states that CB is thought to have a negative affect-reducing function in the short term.17 For example in OCD, it is postulated by Rachman7 that people repeatedly check for safety in situations where they feel unsure about the absence of harm in order to gain relief from their indisposition, uncertainty and anxiety.7 In EDs, it is hypothesised that body checking reduces negative affect which is triggered, for example, by dysfunctional body-related information processing.22 For BDD, it is postulated that there is distress caused by physical appearance, which becomes very strong, for example, in social situations. CB, according to the theory, serves to reduce these unpleasant emotions (eg, fear, disgust, anger, shame) caused by appearance.23 By providing immediate short-term relief from unpleasant feelings, the third theoretical assumption is based on a learning theory mechanism.23 It is postulated that CB as a behaviour is negatively reinforced (ie, produces the absence of a negative consequence), therefore increasing the likelihood that it will be performed more frequently in the future,22 as patients experience CB to be helpful and necessary in the short run. In contrast, theories postulate that repeated use of CB reinforces anxiety and psychopathology in the long term turning into a self-perpetuating mechanism.24 For example, it is discussed that CB can foster distorted perception and evaluation of one’s body in EDs.16 22 For BDD, it is postulated that CB increases selective attention in the long term and may intensify the dysfunctional beliefs about the supposed flaw(s), thus contributing to the maintenance of the disorder.17 19
In sum, although CB looks phenomenologically different depending on the respective disorder, aetiological models across disorders outline checking as an important behaviour, which provides immediate relief from negative states in the short term, therefore reinforcing itself and leading to a self-perpetuating mechanism, and hence contributing to the maintenance of the pathology in the long term. Although the mechanism of action of CB has been postulated in numerous models of different disorders, empirical support for these assumptions is lacking. To date, several empirical studies have investigated the proposed mechanisms in each disorder, but a systematic overview of studies is yet to be undertaken. A systematic overview, however, is urgently needed, given that current cognitive-behavioural treatments for these disorders are based on the afore-mentioned theories and include ritual prevention (ie, not using CB to learn that situations can be handled without this safety behaviour) as one therapeutic technique aimed at reducing CB and consequently related disorder-specific symptoms. Usually, this is addressed in the context of exposure therapy (ie, confronting patients with fearful or even avoided situations without the use of safety behaviours, eg, in OCD, leaving the house without checking the stove and windows). Depending on whether or not the empirical evidence supports the proposed emotion regulating mechanism of checking in the cognitive-behavioural models, the focus of these interventions would need to be intensified or altered, respectively. For example, one might assume that CB does not or not only serve to reduce negative affect but also to gain certainty.26 Therefore, it might be worthwhile to address the excessive need for certainty more directly, for example, through cognitive interventions questioning the pursuit of certainty27 or promoting the willingness to experience fear and uncertainty.28 Furthermore, a better understanding of the mechanisms of action underlying CB may also have implications for the prevention of mental disorders (eg, if the proposed long-term negative effect of CB on psychopathology can be supported by empirical evidence, prevention programmes addressing the reduction of checking in healthy individuals or at-risk groups could be developed).
Objectives
As such, our systematic review intends to synthesise existing evidence for the three postulates regarding CB across the mental disorders OCD, GAD, EDs, BDD and IAD. The current study protocol outlines the methods of our investigation and is based on the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis Protocols (PRISMA-P) checklist (online additional file 1). The following research questions will be addressed: (1) Which (emotional) states are people in when engaging in CB? (2) What effect does CB have on emotional, cognitive and disorder-specific outcomes in the short term (ie, directly following CB)? (3) What effect does CB have on emotional, cognitive and disorder-specific outcomes in the long term (ie, after a repeated number of checking episodes)?
Supplemental material
Methods
Our review has been registered with PROSPERO. The planned data selection process runs from January to May 2022.
Inclusion and exclusion criteria
The inclusion and exclusion criteria are shown in table 1.
Inclusion and exclusion criteria
Information sources
The following electronic databases will be searched: PsycINFO, PubMed, PSYNDEX and Scopus. Furthermore, we will screen the bibliographies of relevant articles for additional studies. Additionally, research registries (ClinicalTrials.gov, PROSPERO and the International Clinical Trials Registry Platform of the WHO) will be searched for eligible unpublished studies. The search process will be presented in a PRISMA flow diagram (online additional file 2). It shows whether an article stems from the electronic databases or from further literature research.
Supplemental material
Search strategy
During the design of the search strategy, library staff were on hand to advise us. To generate search terms, we screened reviews and primary studies as well as the respective keywords (using the “Thesaurus of Psychological Index Terms” and “Medical Subject Headings”). The search terms available for selection were presented to and discussed by a group of experienced clinical researchers. Finally, relevant keywords and, if necessary due to lack of indexing, free text words were selected for each disorder. Since CBs have not yet been keyworded, their search was limited to free text words. To reduce irrelevant hits, only studies that included checking terms in the title or abstract were searched. To be implemented in the scientific databases, the disorder-related search terms were combined using the Boolean operator “AND” with the free text words for Checking. The only filter set is that the search should be limited to studies with human participants. The full search strategy for one database is displayed as additional file (online additional file 3). This will be adapted for each database according to the respective guidelines.
Supplemental material
Study records
Selection process
In a first step, two independent reviewers will screen the titles and abstracts yielded by the search after removal of duplicates. We will then obtain the full text for potentially eligible studies. If the full text is not available, for example, through institutional membership, we will contact the authors to request access. By screening the full text in a second step, the two reviewers will assess for inclusion in the review based on the criteria outlined before. We will note the reason for exclusion of any study and present the selection process in the PRISMA flow diagram (online additional file 2). In the case of discrepancies between the two reviewers in either step, a third reviewer will be consulted. None of the reviewers will be blind to the journal titles or to the study authors or institutions.
Data extraction
For all included studies, data will be extracted by two independent raters using a data collection form developed for this review (online additional file 4). Both reviewers will pilot this in advance with five studies and make adjustments prior to the extraction of data if necessary. We plan to extract the following information and data from each study: (1) basic characteristics of the study: authors, title, publication year, country; (2) sample: sample size, average age, gender, type of sample (clinical vs analogue), diagnosis and criteria for diagnosis (clinical samples) or type of symptoms in analogue samples, comorbidities; (3) setting (eg, online survey, laboratory experiment); (4) type of CB investigated; (5) assessment time points; (6) instruments for the assessment of outcomes and type of outcomes investigated; and (7) study results with regard to the research questions.
Supplemental material
Data synthesis
Selected studies will first be assigned according to the disorder or psychopathology they investigated. Within these five groups, studies will additionally be categorised according to which research question they address. Due to the expected low number of eligible studies, we will carry out a narrative synthesis and compile a table outlining characteristics and findings of every study.
Risk of bias
We will assess the risk of bias within randomised trials using the Cochrane Collaboration tool for assessing risk of bias29 and within non-randomised studies with the Risk Of Bias In Non-randomized Studies of Interventions tool.30 The strength of evidence collected from each study in the review will be assessed using the Grading of Recommendations Assessment, Development and Evaluation system.31 The evaluation process will be conducted by two independent reviewers. If necessary, a third reviewer will resolve disagreements.
Patients and public involvement
Patients and the public will not be directly involved in the design, interpretation or dissemination of the results.
Ethics and dissemination
This systematic review will be based on previously published data, so there will be no requirement for ethical approval. The results of the review will be submitted for publication in a peer-reviewed psychological journal. In addition, the results will be disseminated in various media such as symposia, congresses and seminars.
Ethics statements
Patient consent for publication
Acknowledgments
We want to thank Dr Jost Hindersmann (subject librarian), Wibke Meyer zu Westerhausen and Carin Tholen-Wandel (information specialists) for their advice during the design of the search strategy. We are also thankful to Sarah Mannion de Hernandez for language editing services as well as Antonia Lucht and Lotta Flechsig for the support in the management of literature.
References
Supplementary materials
Supplementary Data
This web only file has been produced by the BMJ Publishing Group from an electronic file supplied by the author(s) and has not been edited for content.
Footnotes
Contributors SV and ASH developed the primary idea for the review, and this was refined with the help of M-BV and VO. All authors contributed to the development of the protocol document. All authors read and agreed the final version of the manuscript. M-BV is the guarantor of the review.
Funding This work was supported by the German Research Foundation (Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft) to ASH (HA 8589/5-1) and SV (VO1750/5-1).
Competing interests None declared.
Patient and public involvement Patients and/or the public were not involved in the design, or conduct, or reporting, or dissemination plans of this research.
Provenance and peer review Not commissioned; externally peer reviewed.
Supplemental material This content has been supplied by the author(s). It has not been vetted by BMJ Publishing Group Limited (BMJ) and may not have been peer-reviewed. Any opinions or recommendations discussed are solely those of the author(s) and are not endorsed by BMJ. BMJ disclaims all liability and responsibility arising from any reliance placed on the content. Where the content includes any translated material, BMJ does not warrant the accuracy and reliability of the translations (including but not limited to local regulations, clinical guidelines, terminology, drug names and drug dosages), and is not responsible for any error and/or omissions arising from translation and adaptation or otherwise.