Article Text

Original research
Associations between daily screen time and sleep in a racially and socioeconomically diverse sample of US infants: a prospective cohort study
  1. Jennifer A Emond1,2,
  2. A James O'Malley1,3,
  3. Brian Neelon4,
  4. Richard M Kravitz5,
  5. Truls Ostbye6,
  6. Sara E Benjamin-Neelon7
  1. 1Department of Biomedical Data Science, Geisel School of Medicine, Dartmouth College, Hanover, New Hampshire, USA
  2. 2Department of Pediatrics, Geisel School of Medicine, Dartmouth College, Hanover, New Hampshire, USA
  3. 3The Dartmouth Institute, Geisel School of Medicine, Dartmouth College, Hanover, New Hampshire, USA
  4. 4Department of Public Health Sciences, Medical University of South Carolina, Charleston, South Carolina, USA
  5. 5Division of Pediatric Pulmonary and Sleep Medicine, Duke University Medical Center, Durham, North Carolina, USA
  6. 6Department of Family Medicine and Community Health, Duke University, Durham, North Carolina, USA
  7. 7Department of Health, Behavior and Society, Johns Hopkins Bloomberg School of Public Health, Baltimore, Maryland, USA
  1. Correspondence to Dr Jennifer A Emond; Jennifer.A.Emond{at}Dartmouth.edu

Abstract

Objective To determine the associations between screen media use and sleep throughout infancy (3–12 months).

Design Prospective Nurture birth cohort.

Setting North Carolina, USA, 2013–2015.

Participants Women enrolled in their second to third trimester, completed a phone interview after birth, and completed home visits at 3, 6, 9 and 12 months post partum.

Primary and secondary outcome measures Women reported the usual hours their infants slept during the day and night and their infants’ usual use of five screen media activities at 3, 6, 9 and 12 months post partum. Adjusted mixed-effects regression analyses modelled the associations between infant screen time and sleep outcomes while disaggregating the between-infant and within-infant effects.

Results Among 558 mother–infant dyads, 374 (67.0%) infants were black and 304 (54.5%) households earned <$20 000 per year. Half (254, 50.2%) of the infants engaged with screens at 3 months of age, while 326 (72.9%) engaged at 12 months. The median value of the average daily screen time over the study period was 50 (IQR: 10–141) min. Infant screen time was inversely associated with night-time sleep duration only when considering between-infant effects (adjusted beta: −2.9; 95% CI −5.9 to 0.0; p=0.054 for log-transformed screen time). Effects were stronger for television+DVD viewing specifically (adjusted beta: −5.2; 95% CI −9.1 to −1.4; p<0.01 for log-transformed television+DVD time). For example, an infant who averaged 1 hour of television+DVD viewing over the study period slept, on average, 9.20 (95% CI 9.02 to 9.37) hours per night by 12 months compared with 9.60 (95% CI 9.41 to 9.80) hours per night for an infant with no screen time over the study period. There were no significant within-infant effects between screen time and night-time sleep, and screen time was not associated with daytime sleep or night-time awakenings.

Conclusions Screen time during infancy was inversely associated with night-time sleep duration; however, causal associations remain uncertain.

Trial registration number NCT01788644.

  • paediatrics
  • public health
  • sleep medicine

Data availability statement

Data are available upon reasonable request.

http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/4.0/

This is an open access article distributed in accordance with the Creative Commons Attribution Non Commercial (CC BY-NC 4.0) license, which permits others to distribute, remix, adapt, build upon this work non-commercially, and license their derivative works on different terms, provided the original work is properly cited, appropriate credit is given, any changes made indicated, and the use is non-commercial. See: http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/4.0/.

Statistics from Altmetric.com

Request Permissions

If you wish to reuse any or all of this article please use the link below which will take you to the Copyright Clearance Center’s RightsLink service. You will be able to get a quick price and instant permission to reuse the content in many different ways.

Strengths and limitations of this study

  • This study reports on the associations between screen use and sleep among infants as they age from 3 to 12 months.

  • Our analytic approach used repeated measures in the study to isolate the effects due to differences between infants versus differences within the same infant over time.

  • Our analyses adjusted for a comprehensive set of potential confounding variables.

  • Racial minorities and those from lower socioeconomic status were well represented in the cohort.

  • The study relied on parent report and thus the precision of the exposure and outcome measures may be limited.

Introduction

Adequate sleep during infancy is essential for healthy cognitive, physical, emotional and behavioural development.1–3 Less night-time sleep has been associated with poorer cognitive development,1 2 socioemotional difficulties3 and overweight/obesity risk4–6 in infants. Infant sleep patterns are highly dynamic in the first year of life. There is wide variability in sleep behaviours up to 3 months of age; thereafter regular circadian rhythms begin to develop.7–9 Infants also shift to more night-time and less daytime sleep and experience fewer night-time awakenings from 4 to 12 months.9–11 In a 2016 consensus statement, the American Academy of Sleep Medicine recommends that infants aged 4–12 months achieve 12–16 hours of sleep per 24 hours.12 Identifying risk factors that may impede the natural development of sleep patterns during infancy is important to ensure optimal growth and development.

There has been limited study of the influence of screen time on sleep during infancy13–18 despite a robust body of research demonstrating adverse effects of excessive screen time (eg, television (TV)/video viewing, playing apps/games) on reduced sleep quantity and quality among children and adolescents.19 20 In 2017, children under the age of 2 years spent 42 min per day engaged with screens, with traditional TV as the primary source.21 In contrast, the American Academy of Pediatrics recommends no screen media for children under 18 months,22 largely due to potential adverse effects on cognitive, socioemotional and language development.23–26 Previous cross-sectional studies have reported inverse associations between screen time and night-time sleep13 18 in infants as young as 4 months. Inverse associations between infant screen time and 24-hour sleep have also been reported,13–18 with mixed findings for infants under 12 months; little research has examined screen use and daytime sleep (with mixed findings)13 18 or night-time awakenings.13

The goal of this study was to examine the associations between infant screen time and sleep quantity during the first year of life, leveraging the repeated-measures data from the prospective Nurture birth cohort study. We present an analytic approach that isolates the associations between screen time and sleep attributable to differences between infants and differences within the same infant over time.27 This approach allows us to address (1) if an infant’s average screen time over the study year is associated with their average sleep over that same time frame; and (2) if an infant’s observed screen time at one timepoint deviates from their expected screen time at that timepoint, their sleep at that timepoint is in turn affected. Using this analytic approach enables more refined statements regarding the associations by comparing an infant’s usual screen time and sleep over the entire study year, as well as shorter-term deviations in those behaviours during a shorter time frame (ie, at each study assessment).

Methods

Participants

The Nurture study28 is a longitudinal, observational birth cohort designed to examine feeding, activity and sleep in relation to excessive weight gain in infancy. Women were recruited from the county health department prenatal clinic and a private prenatal clinic in Durham, North Carolina, USA, during 2013–2015; data collection ended in 2017. Women were enrolled in their second or third trimester, completed a phone interview after birth, and completed home visits at 3, 6, 9 and 12 months post partum, at which time they also completed a questionnaire. A copy of the questionnaire is available from the corresponding author on reasonable request. Women provided written consent at enrolment and verbally confirmed consent for themselves and their infants at birth. The Nurture study was powered at 0.90 probability to detect a 0.09 increase in mean infant weight-for-length z-scores per each 10-hour-per-week increment spent in childcare outside the home, with 666 dyads enrolled and 85% retention over 12 months.28 The original protocol for the study is available in the online supplemental material and is published in BMJ Open.28

Exposures

Women reported their infants’ screen time via questionnaire items at 3, 6, 9 and 12 months as the number of minutes their infants spent doing five screen activities on a typical weekday and typical weekend day, considering their infants’ typical use over the past 4 weeks: (1) watching TV on a TV set; (2) watching DVDs or videotapes; (3) streaming videos or shows on TV, watching on a computer or a handheld device; (4) playing games on a handheld device; or (5) using educational software. Women were instructed to also consider their infants’ screen media exposure at childcare when reporting each value. Weekday and weekend responses were weighted by 5/7 and 2/7, respectively, to compute a mean daily screen time per each category. Totals over all five categories were summed to create the total minutes of screen time per day. We included all screen time, including the use of educational software, to align with the American Academy of Pediatrics’ recommendation of no screen time for children under the age of 18 months.22 Screen time was also categorised as TV+DVD viewing versus other (ie, streaming on a TV, computer or handheld device; playing games on a handheld device; using educational software) to best differentiate between screen time on a traditional TV set versus screen time on mobile devices. Infant screen time was not normally distributed and the distribution was positively skewed. There were several large outlying values and many parents reported no infant screen time. Therefore, screen time values were transformed for analysis. First, values >10 hours per day were truncated at 10 hours per day to align with the maximum allowable values reported in a previous study assessing media use and sleep among children.15 This process affected 55 (2.9%) of all data points for infant screen time. Then, 1 min was added to all values to eliminate zeros and values were natural log-transformed.

Outcomes

Women reported the usual number of hours their infants slept during the day (from 07:00 to 19:00) and night (from 19:00 to 07:00) considering the past 4 weeks via questionnaire items at 3, 6, 9 and 12 months post partum. Durations were summed at each assessment to create total sleep per 24 hours. Women additionally reported the number of times their infants woke in the night (ie, night-time awakenings).

Covariates

Women reported their age, marital status, education and annual household income at enrolment and at each home visit. Infants’ sex, race and ethnicity were reported at the postbirth phone call. Women reported on the following at each postpartum assessment, which were included as potential covariates: number of household members, infant breast feeding (yes/no), and weekly hours of formal and relative-based childcare. Women reported their own screen time at each assessment as the average number of hours they spent watching TV/DVDs/videotapes or streaming on a TV, computer or handheld device, or playing games on a handheld device on a typical weekday and a typical weekend day; whether the mother’s screen time was concurrent with the infant’s screen time was not reported. Daily totals were similarly weighted and summed to create total screen time per day. The number of total media screens at home was also reported at 6 and 12 months post partum. Women reported their current cigarette smoking status (yes/no) and completed the Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale (EPDS)29 at each assessment. EPDS scores of 12 or more were considered a positive screen for postpartum depression.30 To assess overall disorder and environmental confusion at home, women completed the validated Confusion, Hubbub and Order Scale31 at 6 and 12 months post partum; higher scores indicate greater chaos. Potential confounders were selected to account for sociodemographic differences or because of potential associations with infant screen time or infant sleep. Specifically, exclusive breast feeding (vs partial or exclusive formula feeding) has been associated with more night-time awakenings at 3 and 6 months of age,32 more time spent in formal or informal childcare during the day has been inversely associated with night-time sleep duration among infants33 and delayed bedtime among infants and toddlers,34 maternal screen time is highly correlated with infant screen time,35 maternal smoking has been related to night-time awakenings among infants,36 maternal depression has been related to unsettled sleep among infants,37 and a greater level of disorder and chaos at home has been related to sleep problems and fragmented sleep among young children.38 We included a category of ‘missing’ for each covariate to account for missing data. Covariates affected were maternal smoking status (9.3% of dyads were missing data at all timepoints), maternal depression (10.3% of dyads missing data at all timepoints) and maternal screen time (11.6% of dyads missing data at all timepoints). We did not account for missing data for childcare measures because dyads missing data on those measures for all timepoints were minimal (<0.5%). Our results from the adjusted regression models were similar when accounting for the missing data (as presented in the following section) and when the sample was limited to complete cases only (ie, without accounting for missing data), supporting that data on these covariates were missing completely at random. Additionally, values of ‘missing’ for these covariates were not related to the outcome in any model.

Statistical analyses

Analyses were completed on the subset of mother–infant dyads with both infant sleep and screen time reported for at least one postpartum assessment (N=558). We first completed a series of unadjusted analyses to identify potential covariates for the final models. We assessed the associations of infant, maternal and household characteristics with infant screen time (any vs none), and separately the associations of infant, maternal and household characteristics with each sleep outcome using unadjusted bivariate tests: χ2 test when comparing two categorical measures, t-test or one-way analysis of variance when comparing a continuous measure across a categorical measure, and Pearson’s correlation coefficients when comparing two continuous measures.

Generalised mixed-effects regression was used to model the associations between infant screen time and each sleep outcome. Linear regression was used for night-time, daytime and 24-hour sleep, and Poisson regression was used for the number of night-time awakenings. Infant screen time was included in each model in two ways. One, to compare screen time between infants (ie, between-infant effects), each infant’s mean amount of screen time over the 1-year study period was included in the model. Second, to assess how differences in screen time within each infant over the study period related to sleep (ie, within-infant effects), we included a variable in the model that reflected each infant’s deviation in screen time at each timepoint (ie, at 3, 6, 9 or 12 months of age) from their expected value of screen time at that timepoint. Specifically, the method of detrending27 was used, where a simple linear regression model was fit for each infant, regressing screen time on age (3, 6, 9 and 12 months), and residuals from that model reflected each infant’s deviation in their expected value of screen time at each timepoint (ie, the difference between their observed screen time and their expected value based on their own linear regression). Including both of those measures of infant screen time in the model allowed us to isolate the between-infant and within-infant effects, respectively.27 A generalised form of the equation is included in the online supplemental material.

In each mixed-effects regression model, a random intercept and slope at age (ie, the time variable) were included for each infant. All models were adjusted for infant age and time-invariant sociodemographic characteristics measured at baseline (infant sex and race, mother’s age at birth and educational level); annual household income was included as a time-varying covariate. A series of models further adjusted for three sets of time-varying covariates: infant breastfeeding status, maternal characteristics (smoking status, depression and daily screen time) and childcare measures (hours per day of relative or formal childcare, separately). The time-invariant covariates included were those measures associated with infant screen time or any sleep outcome at the p<0.10 level in unadjusted analyses and were grouped into sets to determine how the main effect of infant screen time differed based on adjustment for different risk factors for poor sleep. A set of analyses were also completed to confirm that including infant screen time as log-transformed, versus non-transformed, improved the model fit as judged by Akaike’s information criterion and inspecting the model assumptions (eg, homoscedasticity of residuals for linear regression models). A series of sensitivity analyses were also completed that confirmed there were no significant cross-lagged effects between infant screen media and sleep over time (eg, from 3 to 6 months of age), thus supporting our modelling approach to isolate the between-infant and within-infant effects. The adjusted models were also repeated among the dyads with complete data at each of the four postpartum study timepoints (ie, with infant sleep and screen time data reported at 3, 6, 9 and 12 months post partum). Analyses were completed using the R language for statistical computing (V.3.6.2).

Patient and public involvement

There was no patient or public involvement in the development of the research questions or in the analyses.

Results

Among the 666 women who consented at birth with infants who met the inclusion criteria, 108 were excluded because they did not have any follow-up data on infant sleep or screen time for at least one postpartum visit, leaving 558 dyads in the analytic sample. Dyads included in the analysis were more likely to be white (16.3% vs 9.3%; p=0.02) and women were more likely to have some education past high school (53.8% vs 43.9%; p=0.08) than dyads not included. The distribution of infant sex also differed between those included (49.5% male) versus excluded (60.2% male) from the analysis (p=0.05). Included and excluded dyads were similar by other baseline measures at the p≥0.10 threshold.

Table 1 includes the characteristics of the sample. There was an equal proportion of male and female infants in the sample, most (67.0%) infants were black, the mean maternal age at enrolment was 27.5 years, about half (53.8%) of women completed schooling after high school, and the annual household income was ≤$20 000 for 54.5% of dyads. Most (452, 81.0%) infants engaged with screens during at least one follow-up period (ie, at 3, 6, 9 or 12 months), with a median value for the average daily screen time over the entire postpartum study period of 50 (IQR: 10–141) min per day. Any screen time was more common among black infants and was associated with lower maternal age, non-marital or cohabitation status, lower maternal educational level, cigarette smoking, greater amount of maternal screen time, presence of postnatal depression among mothers at any study timepoint and lower annual household income (table 1).

Table 1

Infant, maternal and household characteristics overall and stratified by any infant screen time during the 12-month postpartum follow-up

The mean (SD) age of infants at each postpartum assessment was 3.4 (0.5), 6.2 (0.5), 9.4 (0.5) and 12.3 (0.8) months, respectively. The proportion of infants with any screen time increased with age (table 2), from 50.2% of infants at 3 months to 72.9% at 12 months (p<0.01).

Table 2

Unadjusted trends in infant screen time during the 12-month postpartum follow-up

Watching traditional TV or DVDs/videotapes accounted for most (>70%) of infant screen time at any assessment. Streaming accounted for 15.5% and 15.7% of infant screen time at 3 and 12 months of age, respectively, and engagement with educational software accounted for 2.6% and 5.4% of screen time at 3 and 12 months of age, respectively. The median infant screen time over the 1-year study period was 50 (IQR: 10–141) min per day (also noted in table 1), with a mean of 109 min per day; the median infant TV+DVD time over the 1-year study period was 36 min per day, with a mean of 79 min per day. The disparities in the median and mean values reflect the positive skew of the data, yet we report the mean for comparison with other studies. When examining time-varying characteristics and infant screen time, the following were positively associated with any infant screen time over the study period: maternal smoking, depression and screen time and infant hours per week in relative-based childcare. Conversely, infant breast feeding and infant hours per week in formal childcare were inversely associated with any infant screen time. There were also significant temporal trends in all sleep outcomes (table 3). On average, infants increased their night-time sleep duration and decreased their daytime and 24-hour sleep duration with age. The number of infant night-time awakenings decreased with age.

Table 3

Infant sleep outcomes by infant age during the 12-month postpartum follow-up

There was a significant between-infant association between total infant screen time (min/day) and night-time sleep duration in the adjusted linear mixed-effects regression models (table 4), where greater values of daily infant screen time over the study year were associated with shorter night-time sleep duration. The effect was stable across the varying adjustments for risk factors; however, the main effect was the most attenuated once adjusted for childcare characteristics relative to the model only adjusted for sociodemographic measures. Model coefficients require transformation for interpretation on the scale of the outcome. Figure 1 presents the predicted (ie, marginal) values from the adjusted regression model that included the childcare covariates (table 4, model 4) to facilitate interpretation of the effect size. For example, infants who averaged 15 min of daily screen time over the study period experienced 13.6 min less night-time sleep per night compared with infants with no screen time, and infants who averaged 60 min of daily screen time over the study period experienced 20.1 min less night-time sleep per night compared with infants with no screen time. An infant who averaged 1 hour of screen time per day over the study period would achieve, on average, 9.28 (95% CI 9.13 to 9.42) hours of night-time sleep at 12 months of age, as compared with 9.61 (95% CI 9.42 to 9.80) hours of night-time sleep for an infant with no screen time over the study period.

Figure 1

Predicted, adjusted values of night-time sleep, stratified by infant age, for a range of mean daily screen time (all screen time combined) over the study period; infants were enrolled in a prospective cohort study. Predicted values are the marginal values from a mixed-effects linear regression model adjusted for sociodemographic characteristics (infant age, sex and race; maternal age, marital/cohabitation status and education; and annual household income) and childcare characteristics (hours/week in relative-based or formal childcare, separately). Marital/cohabitation status, annual household income and childcare characteristics were time-varying covariates. Random effects at the infant level and a random slope for age were included in the model. Infant screen time was included as log-transformed and included as two terms to account for the between-infant and within-infant effects. The predicted values in this figure reflect the between-infant differences in night-time sleep for various values of screen time (min/day) among infants, with screen time reflecting the mean daily screen time per day over the study period (ie, the between-infant differences; see table 4, model 4). Data are from 558 dyads with data collected during at least one postpartum follow-up; not all dyads have complete data at each timepoint. The following are the number of infants for each range of average daily screen time over the study period: 0 min/day: n=106; 1–15 min/day: n=66; 16–30 min/day: n=52; 31–45 min/day: n=42; 46–60 min/day: n=39; 61–75 min/day: n=25; 76–90 min/day: n=30; 91–105 min/day: n=22; 106–120 min/day: n=19; 120+ min/day: n=157.

Table 4

Adjusted longitudinal associations of total infant screen time and night-time sleep while disaggregating the between-infant and within-infant effects

There were no significant within-infant effects in those models, meaning that each infant’s deviation in screen time at any one timepoint, relative to their expected values of screen time at that timepoint, was unrelated to their night-time sleep. Infant race was also strongly associated with night-time sleep duration (table 4), where infants of black race averaged nearly 1 hour less night-time sleep duration as compared with their white peers. Model findings were similar when limited to the 361 dyads with infant screen time and sleep reported at each postpartum timepoint (online supplemental table 1), in that there were significant between-infant effects, yet the within-infant effects were non-significant. For example, the adjusted association for the between-infant effect of daily total screen time and night-time sleep was −4.0 (95% CI −7.9 to −0.1; p<0.05 for log-transformed screen time) and the adjusted association for the within-infant effect of daily total screen time and night-time sleep was 1.1 (95% CI −2.1 to 4.4; p=0.50 for log-transformed screen time); the model was adjusted for sociodemographic and childcare characteristics.

Table 5 presents the model results for the adjusted associations between infant screen time and night-time, daytime and 24-hour sleep for all infant screen time combined and for infant screen time stratified as TV+DVD time and other screen time. The inverse main effect between infant screen time and night-time sleep was stronger when considering infant TV+DVD time specifically (adjusted beta: −5.2; 95% CI −9.1 to −1.4; p<0.01 for log-transformed TV+DVD time), independent of other screen time. Additionally, more TV+DVD screen time was related to less 24-hour sleep duration (adjusted beta: −6.4; 95% CI −12.0 to −0.9; p=0.02 for log-transformed TV+DVD time). Other screen time was also positively associated with more 24-hour sleep duration (adjusted beta: 5.4; 95% CI −0.6 to 11.3; p=0.076 for log-transformed other screen time), although this finding was of borderline statistical significance. Model findings again were similar when limited to the 361 dyads with infant screen time and sleep reported at each of the four postpartum timepoints (online supplemental table 2), in that there were significant between-infant effects, yet the within-infant effects were non-significant. For example, the adjusted association for the between-infant effect of daily TV+DVD time and night-time sleep was −5.2 (95% CI −10.1 to −0.3; p<0.05 for log-transformed TV+DVD time) and the adjusted association for the within-infant effect of daily TV+DVD time and night-time sleep was 1.2 (95% CI −2.5 to 4.9; p=0.52 for log-transformed TV+DVD time); the model was adjusted for other infant screen time and sociodemographic and childcare characteristics. However, in contrast to the model in table 5, the adjusted association for the between-infant effect of TV+DVD time and 24-hour sleep was not statistically significant in the model among the subsets with complete data (adjusted beta: −5.5; 95% CI −12.4 to 1.5; p=0.12 for log-transformed TV+DVD time).

Table 5

Adjusted longitudinal associations between infant screen time and night-time, daytime and 24-hour sleep duration among infants, while disaggregating the between-infant and within-infant effects, with screen time modelled as (A) any screen time combined or (B) stratified as TV+DVD or other screen time

Figure 2 presents the predicted, adjusted values of the sleep outcomes from those models to demonstrate the magnitude of the effect attributable to TV+DVD screen time; the predicted values are the marginal values from the final regression model for various values of infant screen time. Values for sleep outcomes are presented for 12 months of age. Infants who averaged 60 min of daily TV+DVD time over the study period experienced 24.4 min less night-time sleep per night compared with infants with no TV+DVD screen time over the study, adjusted for screen time other than TV+DVD time (figure 2A). There were no significant associations between infant screen time and daytime sleep duration (figure 2B). Infants who averaged 60 min of daily TV+DVD time over the study period experienced 29.1 min less 24-hour sleep compared with infants with no TV+DVD screen time over the study, adjusted for screen time other than TV+DVD time (figure 2C). Finally, there were no associations between infant screen time and the number of night-time awakenings when considering all screen time combined or stratified by TV+DVD and other screen time (data not shown).

Figure 2

Predicted, adjusted mean values of (A) night-time, (B) daytime and (C) 24-hour sleep at 12 months of age, by average daily television (TV)+DVD screen time over the study period. Values are adjusted for other infant screen time. Predicted values are the marginal values from separate mixed-effects linear regression model (n=555) adjusted for sociodemographic characteristics (infant age, sex and race; maternal age, marital/cohabitation status and education; and annual household income) and childcare characteristics (hours/week in relative-based or formal childcare, separately). Marital/cohabitation status, annual household income and childcare characteristics were time-varying covariates. Random effects at the infant level and a random slope for age were included in the model. TV+DVD time was included as a log-transformed variable in the regression model and was included as two terms to account for the between-infant and within-infant effects; each model was also adjusted for all other (ie, non-TV+DVD) screen time. The predicted values in this figure reflect the mean values for each sleep outcome for various values of TV+DVD screen time (min/day), with TV+DVD screen time being the average over the entire 1-year study period (ie, between-infant effects). Data are from 558 dyads with data collected during at least one postpartum follow-up; not all dyads have complete data at each timepoint. The following are the number of infants per each range of average daily TV+DVD time over the study period: 0 min/day: n=124; 1–15 min/day: n=82; 16–30 min/day: n=52; 31–45 min/day: n=47; 46–60 min/day: n=50; 61–75 min/day: n=26; 76–90 min/day: n=24; 91–105 min/day: n=22; 106–120 min/day: n=12; 120+ min/day: n=119.

Discussion

In this diverse sample of 558 mother–infant dyads, most infants engaged with screens from 3 to 12 months of age, and infant screen time was associated with less night-time sleep in the first year of life. Infants who averaged more daily screen time over the study year experienced less night-time sleep than their peers over that same time frame, and the effect was greatest when considering TV+DVD viewing. Specifically, infants who averaged 60 min of daily TV+DVD time over the study period experienced approximately 24 min less night-time and 29 min less 24-hour sleep compared with infants with no TV+DVD screen time during infancy (ie, 3–12 months of age). Effects were consistent when considering adjustment for multiple covariates. Importantly, we did not find evidence of within-infant effects, meaning that deviations in screen time at any one specific timepoint (ie, 3, 6, 9 or 12 months of age), as compared with an infant’s average screen time over the study period, were not uniquely related to sleep. Our findings align with a previous study among older children15 that demonstrated a significant association between greater TV viewing and shorter sleep duration as children aged from 1 to 7 years, where that effect was largely attributable to between-child differences that represented children’s average TV viewing over the study period. In summary, our findings document that screen time increases considerably during infancy and suggest that more screen time, on average, in infancy relates to less night-time sleep, on average. Effects were limited to between-infant differences. In contrast, shorter-term deviations in screen use did not have a more acute effect on sleep, independent of an infant’s usual screen time over the study year.

Our reported inverse association between screen time and night-time sleep duration aligns with the few previous studies among infants13 17 18 and the multitude of studies among preschool-aged and older children.19 In a cross-sectional study of 4-month-old infants,18 each hour of screen use was related to 13 min less night-time sleep duration. In a cross-sectional study of 715 parents of children aged 6–36 months old,13 each additional hour of infant touchscreen tablet use related to 26.4 min less night-time sleep duration; that study did not include the child’s TV viewing but did adjust for background TV exposure. It is difficult to directly compare our findings with those previous studies because of the way infant screen time was treated: in the two previous studies,13 18 infant screen time was treated as a continuous, normally distributed variable, while in our study infant screen time was zero-inflated and highly skewed and was therefore natural log-transformed. Yet effect sizes across the studies are similar. We did not find evidence that infant screen time related to daytime sleep duration. That null association aligns with the previous study of 4-month-olds,18 yet differs from the survey of children aged 6–36 months old,13 in which each additional hour of infant screen time related to 10.8 more minutes of daytime sleep. We also report that infant screen time was unrelated to night-time awakenings, a finding similar to that same previous study of children aged 6–36 months old.13 In summary, evidence appears to align that greater levels of infant screen time are associated with shorter night-time sleep duration, yet associations with other sleep outcomes are less consistent. However, additional studies specifically assessing between-infant and within-infant effects are needed to better understand if our findings are generalisable across different populations.

TV+DVD viewing accounted for most of infants’ screen time in our study, and we found that infant TV+DVD viewing had a greater association with shorter night-time sleep duration than all screen time combined. Our measure of screen time other than TV+DVD viewing approximated mobile or handheld media, media that may have been more challenging for parents to report accurately as compared with TV+DVD viewing, and thus may have been more affected by reporting bias. Mobile media use among infants also requires parental (or other caregiver) involvement and may involve more active interaction from the infant. Thus, mobile screen use may be qualitatively different from when infants more passively view TV.

Our findings suggest that children with higher levels of screen time, on average, over the study period had lower amounts of night-time sleep, on average. Our study cannot prove a causal relationship exists between screen time and sleep. It is possible that reverse causality explains the reported association, where less night-time sleep led to more screen time. It is also possible that unmeasured confounding is related to both increased infant screen time and decreased night-time sleep, which would explain our observed association for the between-infant effect. However, our finding of a significant between-infant effect was consistent after adjusting for a multitude of other risk factors, suggesting unmeasured confounding may be low. The lack of a significant within-infant effect, where deviations in screen time at any one specific timepoint (ie, 3, 6, 9 or 12 months of age), as compared with an infant’s average screen time over the study period, were not uniquely related to sleep, further suggests that more acute and thus potentially causal associations are not present between screen time and sleep. However, our measurements of infant screen time and sleep were both parent-reported and may lack the precision needed to measure small to moderate changes in screen time or sleep over time. Thus, it is possible that measurement error obscured any within-infant effects over the study period. Importantly, negative effects of screen time on infant sleep are biologically plausible.19 Screen time at night or in the bedroom may displace sleep, exposure to stimulating media content may disrupt sleep,39 40 and exposure to light emitted from screens may disrupt sleep hormones and circadian cycles. Unfortunately, we did not record when infants used screens, and we are not able to address if screen time more (vs less) proximal to bedtime accounted for our findings.

This study included high racial and socioeconomic diversity, multiple measures of infant screen time and sleep during the first year, five screen-based activities common among infants, and a rich set of measures to adjust for potential confounding. Our study is not without limitations, however. Infant screen time and sleep were parent-reported and are subject to error, the effect of which may be most pronounced for night-time awakenings.41 We did not collect data on the length of night-time awakenings and thus could not adjust night-time and 24-hour sleep for awakenings. Our measurements of infant screen time lacked the granularity needed to address timing of screen use (eg, before bedtime) and effects on sleep; parents did not report when during the day or night their infants used screens. However, our measure of infant screen time is consistent with previous studies on infant screen time and sleep (ie, usual hours per day).13–18 We also did not assess potentially different effects of different types of content, although children may not comprehend screen media content until 2 years of age.26 It is possible that unmeasured characteristics, such as child temperament,42 43 may confound our reported associations. Per the methods of Curran and Bauer,27 we used simple linear regression to compute each infant’s expected value of screen time at each timepoint, which does not account for the variability of those estimates in the later mixed-effects regression models. Data on both infant screen time and sleep were not available at each timepoint for each dyad; while mixed-effects regression accounts for partial missing data, our effects may be biased if the missing data process was not missing at random.

Conclusions

In this diverse, regional cohort, most infants engaged with screens from 3 to 12 months of age and infant screen time increased with age. Given that screen use behaviours track from infancy into early childhood,44 the findings highlight the need for clear messaging and intervention efforts to limit screen use and exposure during infancy. Furthermore, we found that greater amounts of infant screen time were associated with shorter night-time sleep duration when considering the between-infant effect, yet we report a non-significant within-infant effect. Studies to specifically assess potential causality, ideally with more intensive objective measures of infant sleep and screen time, are needed to help inform evidence-based guidelines. Continued studies are needed to understand how screen use at such a young age may affect sleep and other areas of growth and development, and our findings support the current guidelines of no screen time for infants under 18 months of age to support healthy sleep behaviours in infancy given the potential detrimental effects of screen time on infant health.

Data availability statement

Data are available upon reasonable request.

Ethics statements

Ethics approval

Ethical approval for this study was received from the Institutional Review Board of Duke University Medical Center.

Acknowledgments

We thank the women and infants enrolled in the Nurture study for their participation.

References

Supplementary materials

  • Supplementary Data

    This web only file has been produced by the BMJ Publishing Group from an electronic file supplied by the author(s) and has not been edited for content.

Footnotes

  • Contributors JAE and SEB-N had full access to all the data in the study and take responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis. TO, RK and SEB-N designed the initial cohort study, obtained funding and acquired the data. JAE, AJO and SEB-N conceptualised and designed the current secondary analysis. JAE analysed the data and AJO, BN and SEB-N provided critical input during data analysis. JAE drafted the manuscript and all authors provided critical revision to the manuscript for important intellectual content. All authors read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

  • Funding This study was supported by a grant from the National Institutes of Health (R01DK094841) and a mentored research scientist training award from the National Institutes of Health (5K01DK117971). The sponsors had no role in the design and conduct of the study; collection, management, analysis and interpretation of the data; preparation, review or approval of the manuscript; or decision to submit the manuscript for publication.

  • Competing interests None declared.

  • Provenance and peer review Not commissioned; externally peer reviewed.

  • Supplemental material This content has been supplied by the author(s). It has not been vetted by BMJ Publishing Group Limited (BMJ) and may not have been peer-reviewed. Any opinions or recommendations discussed are solely those of the author(s) and are not endorsed by BMJ. BMJ disclaims all liability and responsibility arising from any reliance placed on the content. Where the content includes any translated material, BMJ does not warrant the accuracy and reliability of the translations (including but not limited to local regulations, clinical guidelines, terminology, drug names and drug dosages), and is not responsible for any error and/or omissions arising from translation and adaptation or otherwise.