Article Text

Napping, development and health from 0 to 5 years: a systematic review
  1. Karen Thorpe1,
  2. Sally Staton1,
  3. Emily Sawyer2,
  4. Cassandra Pattinson1,
  5. Catherine Haden3,
  6. Simon Smith4
  1. 1School of Psychology and Counselling, Institute for Health and Biomedical Innovation, Queensland University of Technology, Queensland, Australia
  2. 2School of Medicine and Dentistry, James Cook University, Queensland, Australia
  3. 3Department of Library, Queensland University of Technology, Queensland, Australia
  4. 4Centre for Accident Research & Road Safety—Queensland, Institute for Health and Biomedical Innovation, Queensland University of Technology, Queensland, Australia
  1. Correspondence to Professor Karen Thorpe, School of Psychology and Counselling, Level 5, O Block, B Wing, Queensland University of Technology, Victoria Park Rd, Kelvin Grove, Queensland 4051, Australia; k.thorpe{at}qut.edu.au

Abstract

Background Duration and quality of sleep affect child development and health. Encouragement of napping in preschool children has been suggested as a health-promoting strategy.

Objectives The aim of this study is to assess evidence regarding the effects of napping on measures of child development and health.

Design This study is a systematic review of published, original research articles of any design.

Subjects Children aged 0–5 years.

Method Electronic database search was performed following Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses guidelines and assessment of research quality was carried out following a Grading of Recommendations, Assessment, Development and Evaluations (GRADE) protocol.

Results Twenty-six articles met inclusion criteria. These were of heterogeneous quality; all had observational designs (GRADE-low). Development and health outcomes included salivary cortisol, night sleep, cognition, behaviour, obesity and accidents. The findings regarding cognition, behaviour and health impacts were inconsistent, probably because of variation in age and habitual napping status of the samples. The most consistent finding was an association between napping and later onset, shorter duration and poorer quality of night sleep, with evidence strongest beyond the age of 2 years.

Limitations Studies were not randomised. Most did not obtain data on the children's habitual napping status or the context of napping. Many were reliant on parent report rather than direct observation or physiological measurement of sleep behaviour.

Conclusions The evidence indicates that beyond the age of 2 years napping is associated with later night sleep onset and both reduced sleep quality and duration. The evidence regarding behaviour, health and cognition is less certain. There is a need for more systematic studies that use stronger designs. In preschool children presenting with sleep problems clinicians should investigate napping patterns.

  • Sleep
  • Child Psychology
  • Comm Child Health

Statistics from Altmetric.com

Request Permissions

If you wish to reuse any or all of this article please use the link below which will take you to the Copyright Clearance Center’s RightsLink service. You will be able to get a quick price and instant permission to reuse the content in many different ways.

What is already known on this topic?

  • Early childhood is an important period in sleep development in which sleep consolidates into the night and napping ceases.

  • The total duration and quality of sleep in a 24 h period is a predictor of child health and development.

  • In early childhood, children's sleep patterns can disrupt family functioning and parent well-being.

What this study adds?

  • Our review identifies consistent reports that, beyond the age of 2 years, napping is increasingly associated with delayed night sleep onset and disrupted night sleep.

  • Our review identifies the need for more extensive and higher quality studies of the effects of napping on multiple outcomes, including learning, behaviour and health.

  • Extant evidence does not support the practice of prolonging napping behaviour once sleep consolidates into the night.

Introduction

The duration and quality of sleep have immediate, ongoing and long-term consequences for child development and health.1 ,2 During early childhood, sleep patterns affect the individual child and can also have a profound effect on family functioning and parent well-being.3 There is continuing emotive debate about what is optimal parenting practice with regard to sleep during early childhood.4 Although this controversy is not new, recent evidence on the significance of sleep for long-term child health has intensified this debate. Evidence linking sleep duration to health outcomes derives predominantly from studies of night sleep or total sleep in a 24 h period. The findings have been extrapolated to infer the value of promoting napping, in both home and childcare contexts.5 We sought to understand how well this assumption was founded. The purpose of the current review was to examine the state of evidence and current findings regarding the independent effects of napping on children's night sleep, behaviour, cognitive functioning and physical health from birth to 5 years.

Early childhood is a particularly important period in sleep development. From birth to 5 years is a time of normative transition in sleep patterns during which sleep gradually consolidates into the nighttime hours and daytime naps cease.6 This is also a sensitive period in which children's sleep patterns are increasingly responsive to environmental modification.7 The presence of napping across this transition might therefore reflect ongoing natural biphasic sleep patterns or alternative mechanisms such as opportunistic sleep or compensation for inadequate night sleep.5 A further complexity is that sleep achieved during a daytime nap may not be equivalent to that achieved at night, with reports of differences in sleep architecture,8 ,9 circadian timing10 and developmental salience.11

Understanding the independent contribution of napping within the range of sleep factors influencing child development and health is significant to inform parents, non-parental carers and health professionals about optimal sleep practices. The current systematic review was therefore undertaken to assess the evidence regarding costs and benefits of napping for the development and health of children aged 0–5 years.

Methods

A review protocol was developed by the authors using the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses Guidelines.12

Search strategy

An extensive search for relevant studies was carried out on the 8 January 2014 using the following electronic databases: Medline (via Ebscohost), PsycINFO (via Ebscohost), CINAHL (via Ebscohost), Web of Science, Scopus and ERIC (via Ebscohost). Searches were conducted to identify papers that contained the following key words: (“daytime sleep” OR nap* OR “day?sleep” OR “sleep consolidation”) AND (cognition OR behavio?r OR “physical Health” OR “night sleep”) AND (child* OR infant OR bab*). The search strategies were developed in consultation with a Health Liaison Librarian.

The exact search terms and limiters used for each database are listed in the online supplementary table S1. Reference lists from papers identified by searches were also examined to identify potential papers for inclusion.

Inclusion criteria

All published, original research articles that examined the independent effects of napping on night sleep, behaviour, cognition and physical health in children aged 0–5 years were included for review. Napping was defined as periods of sleep, measured using observational, parent/carer or self-report or physiological measurement that occurs during daytime hours. Studies that included napping as part of total or 24 h sleep measurement were included only if the effects of the napping component could be reasonably differentiated from the effects of night-time sleep or if napping was indicated by a ratio of day-to-night time sleep (an index of sleep consolidation).

Selection of studies

A three-step approach to the selection of studies was undertaken. First, two review authors (ES and SSt) independently examined the title and abstract of all records identified via searches to determine whether they met the inclusion criteria. Second, full-text versions of relevant studies were obtained and the same review authors independently examined the details of each to determine whether they meet the predetermined criteria. The rationale for inclusion or exclusion of a study was documented by each review author. Finally, decisions for inclusion and exclusion of full-text articles were discussed with additional members of the research team (KT, SSm and CP) and a consensus method used to resolve any disagreements.

Grading of study quality

To assess the quality of each article, we used a GRADE system.13 ,14 GRADE first assigns a quality score for study design. Randomised control trials are scored as high quality, and observational studies, including longitudinal, quasi-experimental and correlational designs, are scored as low quality. In the study of napping, experimental designs that manipulate napping and longitudinal studies that provide detail of the sequencing of effect of night and day sleep are significant in showing direction of effect. For this reason we increased GRADE score by 1 point for design when studies were experimental or longitudinal. In addition, GRADE deducts or adds points if aspects of the methodology increase or decrease the certainty of the result. In assessing the quality of research for this study, we reduced scores if key methodological features decreased certainty of the findings regarding sleep in early childhood (low objectivity of the sleep measure, low-quality data on habitual napping). We also reduced scores if there were general methodological features that reduced certainty (reporting bias, poor control of confounders, low levels of observation). GRADE also makes provision for increasing scores if high effect size is found; however, as the outcomes and their measurement were diverse, comparison and grading would be arbitrary and were not undertaken and precluded meta-analyses. Instead, for this review we were dependent on consistency of direction of result only. The GRADE criteria for this review are documented in online supplementary table S2. Using these criteria two authors (KT, ES) each graded the studies. The authorship team undertook a final review to ensure consensus.

Results

Database search and data extraction

Figure 1 presents the data extraction records for each stage of the search. Of the initial 781 papers identified after exclusion of duplicates, 26 met the criteria for inclusion. Most papers reported on a single outcome, but two reported on more than one outcome. An overview of the key details of the 26 papers arranged by outcome measure is presented in table 1.

Table 1

Summary of papers

Figure 1

Systematic review search flow diagram based on Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses.9

Designs and methodologies

There were no randomised control trials within the corpus of studies. There was a heterogeneous group of observational studies that included experimental (n=5), quasi-experimental (n=6), longitudinal (n=6) and cross-sectional designs (n=9). The description of each study and their findings are summarised in table 1 and summary of quality and GRADE score in table 2. It should be noted that the sample sizes are generally small and therefore present challenges for generalisability. In addition, the studies using correlational designs had varying levels of statistical control. Where such factors present a major concern for interpretation this is noted in table 2.

Table 2

Quality of papers

Outcomes

The consistency of benefit or cost to the child was examined with reference to study quality. Three studies examining the association of napping with changes in salivary cortisol were not included in this process because the meaning of the outcomes could not be interpreted in terms of benefit or cost to the child. A summary of the direction of findings for all other outcomes is presented in figure 2.

Figure 2

Number of articles reporting health and development benefits or costs of napping.

The most consistent finding was an association between napping and night sleep. Table 3 provides a summary of the magnitude of findings and documents a moderate association of napping with night sleep patterns including later sleep onset,15 ,18 ,19 reduced duration5 ,16 ,17 ,20 and quality indices.16 ,19 ,20 Three studies examine differences in total 24 h sleep duration and report no difference between those who do and do not nap, indicating that daytime napping changes the distribution, rather than the duration, of sleep in a 24 h period.5 ,16 ,20 However, Cairns and Harsh22 in a study of transition to school report reduction in total sleep duration which they attribute to loss of naps. Notably, eight of the studies of nighttime sleep were of children aged >2 years, a point at which there is a significant decline in habitual napping.6 Three papers provide evidence of direction of association. Komada et al18 presents evidence of the direction of the relationship through a detailed study of nine consecutive 24 h periods in a sample of 967 children aged 0–5 years. The study reports that there were no significant differences in night sleep associated with duration of napping in children <2 years of age. Children >2 years of age, however, decreased sleep duration and later bedtime occurred on nights after napping. Fukuda and Sakashita15 report that among children attending Kindergarten, where napping is optional, the onset of night sleep was later on evenings after a nap compared with onset on days in which the same children had not napped. Napping was not found to be a response to shorter sleep duration on the night preceding a nap. Iwata et al39 present data on children who are non-habitual nappers attending nursery during weekdays, where naptime is compulsory and compares their weekday and weekend sleep. During weekends, most children did not nap and had earlier sleep onset and shorter sleep latency. A subgroup of children, however, had late unplanned naps that increased the onset and decreased the quality of their nighttime sleep. A likely explanation for these findings is that napping acts to dissipate homeostatic sleep pressure,10 with this mechanism evidence by increased sleep latency and later sleep onset. Together the evidence suggests that, beyond the age of 2 years napping can influence night sleep patterning.15 ,18 ,39 The meaning of these affects, however, remains uncertain.

Table 3

Summary of the magnitude of significant findings regarding napping and child outcomes

Behavioural outcomes were examined by five studies. Berger et al24 report an experimental study of sleep restriction among children, aged 30–36 months, trained to a nap schedule. Under these conditions a reduction in positive facial emotional responses and increase in negative facial emotional responses to visual stimuli and problem solving were found. Three studies are non-experimental. Spruyt et al,23 in a detailed 12-month longitudinal study, report that children with easier temperament napped more readily, but at 12 months, decreased daytime sleep was associated with better emotional regulation as measured by the Bayley Scales of Infant Development. Two studies report association with internalising behavioural problems using the parent-form of the Child Behaviour Check List.17 ,25 Yokomaku et al17 found longer nap duration among children aged 4–6 years was associated with greater anxiety/depression, withdrawal and thought problems, whereas Hall et al25 found that among children aged 1–3 years difficulty settling for naps at home was associated with anxiety, depression, withdrawal and internalising behaviour. In addition, Hall et al25 obtained teacher report using the Preschool Adjustment Questionnaire and Preschool Behaviour Questionnaire. There were inconsistent outcomes; higher daily frequency of napping was associated with less prosocial behaviour and reduced ability to deal with challenges, whereas difficulty settling for naps was associated with poorer behaviour and poorer adjustment at preschool. The difference between the experimental study and non-experimental studies are most likely explained by two processes. First, although the experimental study focuses on disruption of habitual napping,24 the non-experimental studies focus on normally occurring nap behaviours.17 ,23 ,25 Second, two of the non-experimental studies were of children who attended preschool settings.17 ,25 The preschool setting may be a factor explaining the association of problems settling for naps and behavioural difficulties because these contexts often mandate naptime and may not be environmentally conducive to napping.40 Both the stage in developmental sleep transition and the environmental context of napping are significant factors in the interpretation of results.

Studies of cognitive outcomes also present a mixed picture. Three studies report that language learning is improved after a nap,9 ,26 ,27 whereas another reports that being awake, rather than napping, improves language generalisation.30 The age of children and the task used to assess learning may be implicated in these findings but there is currently insufficient evidence to speculate on cause of this inconsistency. An important factor in interpretation of effect, however, is the habitual napping status of the children. Kurdziel et al9 report a positive effect of napping on learning and memory consolidation in preschool aged children, but with benefit only for habitual nappers. Two further studies suggest that sleep consolidation (ratio of night-to-day sleep) is a key marker of cognitive maturity.20 ,28 Dionne et al,28 using a longitudinal twin design, mapped the association of sleep consolidation from 6 to 30 months on language development at 60 months. They report that later sleep consolidation is a risk factor for language delay, whereas more rapid sleep consolidation positively predicts language learning.28 Similarly, Lam et al20 report that daytime napping was associated with poorer neurocognitive function in children aged 3–5 years and suggest that cessation of napping may be a marker of brain maturation. The corpus of cognition studies directs attention to both the benefits of napping for learning and memory, especially among younger children,26 ,27 and the changing value of napping across time.20 Sleep consolidation may be a global marker of neurological maturity.

Evidence on the implications of napping for physical health was poor. Two studies report that, among preschoolers, napping serves to protect against accidental injury.31 ,35 Both studies, however, have the serious limitation of dependence on retrospective recall at the time of attending accident and emergency departments.31 ,35 The only other published studies pertaining to napping and health were three reporting on the association with childhood obesity.32–34 All reported shorter sleep duration as a predictor of excessive weight or obesity, but no independent effect for napping. These findings suggest that the function of napping with regard to weight status is potentially different from that of night sleep. Considerably more research is required before there can be any certainty about the effects of napping on children's physical health.

In summary, there is currently a low volume of evidence regarding the impacts of napping on children's development and health. Extant literature covers a range of outcomes with few using standard, comparable measures. The quality of studies reflects the relatively new focus of research on the independent effects of napping. There is an imperative for increased understanding about individual differences in napping trajectories and, indeed, sleep needs within this age group. Future studies should address the dual complexities of sleep transition across early childhood and the impacts of environmental manipulations at home and in the non-parental care context and should consider multiple child outcomes.

Conclusion

There is consistent, although low quality, evidence that night sleep onset, duration and quality are influenced by napping, particularly beyond the age of 2 years as sleep consolidates into the nighttime. The relationship between napping and other child development and health outcomes is less clear. Greater certainty about the nature and direction of effect would be provided by stronger study designs, with attention to population representation in sampling. These should include collection of data about habitual napping status and the stability of napping across home and childcare context. Studies should use independent physiological measurement of sleep. The impact of night sleep on children's development and health is increasingly documented, but to date there is not sufficient evidence to indicate the value of prolonging napping, whether at home or in childcare contexts, once sleep has consolidated into the night. For clinicians treating sleep problems, particularly among preschool aged children, the investigation of napping patterns is indicated.

References

View Abstract

Supplementary materials

  • Supplementary Data

    This web only file has been produced by the BMJ Publishing Group from an electronic file supplied by the author(s) and has not been edited for content.

    Files in this Data Supplement:

  • Press release

    This web only file has been produced by the BMJ Publishing Group from an electronic file supplied by the author(s) and has not been edited for content.

    Files in this Data Supplement:

Footnotes

  • Contributors KT conceptualised and designed the study, supervised database searches and screening, analysed and interpreted the data and drafted the final manuscript. SSt conceptualised and designed the study, developed the review protocol, undertook database searches and screening, contributed to interpretation of data, contributed to drafting of the manuscript and critically reviewed the manuscript. ES undertook database searches and screening, assisted in the analysis and interpretation of the data and critically reviewed the manuscript. CP conceptualised and designed the study, contributed to interpretation of the data and critically reviewed the manuscript. CH assisted in the development of search protocols, undertook database searches and contributed to drafting of the manuscript. SSm conceptualised and designed the study, supervised data collection, contributed to interpretation of data and critically reviewed the manuscript. All authors approved the final manuscript as submitted.

  • Competing interests None.

  • Provenance and peer review Not commissioned; externally peer reviewed.

  • Data sharing statement Data from the extraction process is available from authors by request.

Linked Articles