Elsevier

Schizophrenia Research

Volume 78, Issues 2–3, 15 October 2005, Pages 293-296
Schizophrenia Research

Short scales for measuring schizotypy

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2005.06.020Get rights and content

Abstract

Background

This study reports short scales for measuring several dimensions of schizotypy in the normal population based on a large twin sample.

Methods

The four short scales use items drawn from a longer instrument, the Oxford-Liverpool Inventory of Feelings and Experiences. Using concordance estimates from MZ and DZ pairs, the items were selected both to have a high heritability and to offer broad coverage of each trait domain.

Results

Preliminary descriptive statistics are reported for the short scales and suggest adequate reliability.

Conclusions

New scales offer a time efficient and reliable method of studying proneness to psychosis in large N designs.

Introduction

Over the past several decades many self-report scales have been developed for measuring schizotypal personality traits in non-clinical individuals (for reviews see Chapman et al., 1995, Mason et al., 1997b). Factor analytic studies of these measures have most frequently supported either three or four components, depending on the number and item content of scales included in the analyses (Claridge et al., 1996, Vollema and van den Bosch, 1995). The Oxford-Liverpool Inventory of Feelings and Experiences (O-LIFE) is an instrument based on analysis of what is probably the largest single dataset of schizotypal measures (Claridge et al., 1996). It contains four sub-scales called ‘Unusual Experiences’, ‘Cognitive Disorganisation’, ‘Introvertive Anhedonia’ and ‘Impulsive Nonconformity’. Since its introduction, the O-LIFE has enjoyed wide currency. Early work established its high internal consistency (Mason et al., 1995) and test–retest reliability (Burch et al., 1998). It has also been used in a variety of studies across many research domains, firmly establishing its construct validity as a genuine measure of schizotypal traits. Laboratory investigations have demonstrated predictable effects in relation to neuropsychological function (e.g. Avons et al., 2003); on several perceptual and attentional paradigms (e.g. Steel et al., 2002); in psychophysiological responding (e.g. Mason et al., 1997a); on reasoning tasks (e.g. Sellen et al., 2005) and in learning, notably on measures of ‘latent inhibition’ (e.g. Moran et al., 2003). Differences have also been found in hemispheric function: for language task performance (Nunn and Peters, 2001), face processing (Mason and Claridge, 1999), and handedness (Shaw et al., 2001). With clinical relevance, the O-LIFE has been used successfully to investigate schizotypy in relation to such topics as dissociative experience and childhood abuse (Startup, 1999) and paranormal beliefs and experiences as a function of mental health (Goulding, 2004). It is also important that there is evidence for a genetic basis as measures of this type are otherwise open to the criticism that they are merely phenomenologically or, in Meehl's (1993) terms, ‘pseudo-phenotypically’ related to schizophrenia. A recent large quantitative genetic analysis (Linney et al., 2003) has established convincing heritability estimates for the scales. However, at over 100 items, the full scales are arguably cumbersome and a shortened form is very desirable. We felt that it was very important to retain those aspects of schizotypy most relevant to ‘true’ proneness to disorder without losing the broad ‘flavour’ of each trait. By using a twin sample we aimed to retain, and even to maximise, the degree of genotypic variance captured by items without compromising scale content.

Section snippets

Participants

Nine hundred twenty-eight twin pairs (mean age 47) from the Institute of Psychiatry Volunteer Twin Register gave informed consent and completed the measure. None of the participants reported having had a psychotic breakdown. Zygosity was assessed using a series of questions that have been found to discriminate MZ from DZ twins with over 95% reliability (Cederlöf et al., 1961, Sarna and Kaprio, 1980). Seventy were monozygotic male pairs, 468 were monozygotic female pairs, 29 were dizygotic male

Results

The items selected for short scales are given in Table 1. An affirmative answer to each item contributes one point, with reverse scoring for those indicated. Descriptive statistics are reported in Table 2. Their internal consistency was calculated using the alpha coefficient with all four exceeding 0.6. While Nunnally (1978) suggests that coefficients of 0.7 are ideal, he describes 0.6 as an acceptable level of given measurement error in psychological/social science. The concurrent validity was

Discussion

The approach to item selection used Plomin's logic (2000, p.80) that ‘to the extent that identical twin concordances are greater than fraternal twin concordance genetic influence is implied’. However, the method aimed to ensure good reliability, content validity and concurrent validity in addition to retaining heritability, and results supported these. The short scales are intended for research use in non-clinical samples and it is important to highlight that they are not intended

References (24)

  • G. Claridge et al.

    The factor structure of ‘schizotypal’ traits: a large replication study

    Br. J. Clin. Psychol.

    (1996)
  • H.V. Curran et al.

    Cognitive, dissociative, and psychotogenic effects of ketamine in recreational users on the night of drug use and 3 days later

    Addiction

    (2000)
  • Cited by (244)

    • Metacognitive monitoring in schizotypy: Systematic literature review and new empirical data

      2023, Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry
    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text