Original Article
Neural activation in women in response to masculinized male faces: mediation by hormones and psychosexual factors

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2008.08.006Get rights and content

Abstract

Women's preference for masculine faces varies with hormonal state, sociosexuality, and relationship status, but the underlying mechanisms are poorly understood. We hypothesized that hormones and psychosexual factors (sociosexuality, sexual inhibition/excitation) mediate the perception and evaluation of male faces thereby influencing women's preferences. We used functional magnetic resonance imaging to measure brain activity in 12 women as they evaluated pictures of male faces (half 30% masculinized, half 30% feminized). Participants were heterosexual women, age 23–28 years, who were not in a committed relationship and not using hormonal contraception. Women were tested during both the follicular and luteal phase of their menstrual cycle. We found five brain regions related to face and risk processing that responded more to the masculinized than to the feminized faces, including the superior temporal gyrus, precentral gyrus, posterior cingulate cortex, inferior parietal lobule, and anterior cingulate cortex. Increased activation in the anterior cingulate cortex, specifically, may indicate that women perceive masculinized faces to be both more risky and more attractive. We did not see any areas that were more strongly activated by feminized faces. Levels of activation were influenced by hormonal and psychosexual factors. The patterns of hormonally and psychosexually mediated neural activation observed may offer insight into the cognitive processes underlying women's partner preferences.

Introduction

Mate choice is a complex decision with high potential risks for women. Because male traits generally thought to predict good condition and even genetic quality often coincide with less desirable characteristics, women must balance potentially disparate mating priorities (Gangestad & Simpson, 2000, Jones et al., 2008). For example, although men characterized by more masculine testosterone-linked traits (Penton-Voak & Chen, 2004) may be socially dominant (Boothroyd, Jones, Burt, & Perrett, 2007) and physically healthy (Rhodes et al., 2003, Thornhill & Gangestad, 2006), they are also less likely to invest in offspring (Fleming et al., 2002, Roney et al., 2006) and to enter into a partnered relationship (Booth & Dabbs, 1993, Van Anders & Watson, 2006). Facial, morphological, and behavioral cues of masculinity have been previously proposed to be salient cues of risk and reward for women in their assessment of men as potential sexual partners (Miller & Todd, 1998). Although the computation of the relative risks and rewards of a potential mate would be expected to be the product of conscious and unconscious cognitive processing of stimuli in the brain, the neural processes associated with women's evaluations of men during women's sexual decision making and how those processes may relate to preferences for more or less masculine men are not known.

Preferences for facial masculinity fluctuate across the menstrual cycle in parallel with changes in hypothesized reproductive priorities (Gangestad et al., 2004, Gangestad et al., 2007; Penton-Voak & Perrett, 2000). Around the time of ovulation, when conception is likely, women may be more responsive to cues that predict male genetic quality, specifically, traits indicating increased androgens, leading to a preference for traits that are not favored during other phases (Gangestad et al., 2004, Jones et al., 2005, Pawlowski & Jasienska, 2005, Penton-Voak et al., 1999, Penton-Voak & Parrett, 2000). Evidence suggests a direct relationship between specific hormones, including estrogens, testosterone, and progesterone, and women's preferences for certain masculine traits (Roney & Simmons, 2008, Welling et al., 2007). For example, recent work demonstrates direct positive correlations between women's subjective evaluations of masculine men and their testosterone (Welling et al., 2007) and estradiol (Roney & Simmons, 2008). Furthermore, women using hormonal contraception do not demonstrate the same fluctuating patterns of attraction to masculine men as observed in normally cycling women (Penton-Voak et al., 1999). However, although the literature supports an association between hormonal state and women's preferences for more or less masculine men, little is known about how hormones mediate women's mate choice preferences.

We hypothesize that hormones mediate the perception and evaluation of male faces. If true, we would expect to see hormonal effects on neural activation in brain areas involved in face perception, the evaluation of facial attractiveness, and decision making, including the anterior cingulate, insula, amygdala, nucleus accumbens, medial prefrontal cortex, fusiform gyrus, and superior temporal gyrus (Allison et al., 2000, Harris et al., 2007, Haxby et al., 2000, Heekeren et al., 2008, Kranz & Ishai, 2006, O'Doherty et al., 2003, Palermo & Rhodes, 2007, Redcay, 2008, Stevens et al., 2005, Winston et al., 2007). We expect this network of brain regions to be influenced by both the masculinity of face stimuli and the phase of a woman's menstrual cycle at the time of testing based on extensive behavioral literature documenting women's increased preferences for masculine men during the follicular phase (reviewed in Jones et al., 2008). An understanding of women's neural responses to masculine men across the menstrual cycle will further our knowledge of the proximate mechanisms by which hormones alter the reward value and/or the perception of masculine faces.

Section snippets

Participants

A total of 16 heterosexual women were recruited for this study from graduate and professional schools at a large Midwestern university and the surrounding community. Recruitment was accomplished through e-mails and flyers. Twelve of the 16 women successfully completed both test sessions within a 1-month period. Participants were on average 25.15 years old (S.D.=1.91), none reported currently using hormonal contraceptives, none reported any sexual desire disorders (The Brief Index of Sexual

Subjective evaluations

A multivariate 2(phase)×2 (masculinization/feminization) repeated measures ANOVA demonstrated that participants' subjective evaluations of likelihood of having sex with the men presented did not differ by menstrual cycle phase or masculinity of the faces (mean±S.D.=1.92±.29). Correlation analyses (Spearman one-tailed) between participants' subjective ratings for men overall, hormone levels, and psychosexual profiled demonstrated significant positive associations between women's subjective

Discussion

Women demonstrated different neural responses to masculinized versus feminized faces during a sexual decision making task. This is the first study to demonstrate differences in neural activation to masculinized versus feminized faces. Specifically, we found five general brain regions related to face perception, decision making, and reward processing that responded more strongly to masculinized than feminized faces: the left superior temporal gyrus, bilateral precentral gyrus, the right

Acknowledgments

The authors thank Dr. David Perrett and The Perception Laboratory for use of the Psychomorph software. We would also like to thank Dr. Ronald McClintock and the Indiana University General Clinical Research Center (GCRC) for hormone assays (GCRC Grant: M01 RR00750). Finally we gratefully acknowledge Sunah Kim and Ryan Stevenson for programming, analysis, and technical help. This work was supported by the National Institutes of Health-funded Common Themes in Reproductive Diversity training grant

References (59)

  • JonesB.C. et al.

    Commitment to relationships and preferences for femininity and apparent health in faces are strongest on days of the menstrual cycle when progesterone level is high

    Hormones and Behavior

    (2005)
  • KranzF. et al.

    Face perception is modulated by sexual preference

    Current Biology

    (2006)
  • KuhnenC.M. et al.

    The neural basis of financial risk taking

    Neuron

    (2005)
  • LawrenceE.J. et al.

    The role of ‘shared representations’ in social perception and empathy: An fMRI study

    NeuroImage

    (2006)
  • MillerG.F. et al.

    Mate choice turns cognitive

    Trends in Cognitive Sciences

    (1998)
  • O'DohertyJ. et al.

    Beauty in a smile: The role of medial orbitofrontal cortex in facial attractiveness

    Neuropsychologia

    (2003)
  • OinonenK.A. et al.

    Facial symmetry detection ability changes across the menstrual cycle

    Biological Psychology

    (2007)
  • PalermoR. et al.

    Are you always on my mind? A review of how face perception and attention interact

    Neuropsychologia

    (2007)
  • PaulusM.P. et al.

    Increased activation in the right insula during risk-taking decision making is related to harm avoidance and neuroticism

    NeuroImage

    (2003)
  • PawlowskiB. et al.

    Women's preferences for sexual dimorphism in height depend on menstrual cycle phase and expected duration of relationship

    Biological Psychology

    (2005)
  • Penton-VoakI.S. et al.

    High salivary testosterone is linked to masculine male facial appearance in humans

    Evolution and Human Behavior

    (2004)
  • Penton-VoakI.S. et al.

    Female preference for male faces changes cyclically: Further evidence

    Evolution and Human Behavior

    (2000)
  • RedcayE.

    The superior temporal sulcus performs a common function for social and speech perception: Implications for the emergence of autism

    Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews

    (2008)
  • RoneyJ.R. et al.

    Women's estradiol predicts preference for facial cues of men's testosterone

    Hormones and Behavior

    (2008)
  • StevensM.C. et al.

    Low-dose estradiol alters brain activity

    Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging

    (2005)
  • ThornhillR. et al.

    Facial sexual dimorphism, developmental stability, and susceptibility to disease in men and women

    Evolution and Human Behavior

    (2006)
  • Van AndersS.M. et al.

    Relationship status and testosterone in North American heterosexual and non-heterosexual men and women: Cross-sectional and longitudinal data

    Psychoneuroendocrinology

    (2006)
  • WallenK.

    Desire and ability: Hormones and the regulation of female sexual behavior

    Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews

    (1990)
  • WaynforthD. et al.

    The influence of women's mating strategies on preference for masculine facial architecture

    Evolution and Human Behavior

    (2005)
  • Cited by (43)

    • Social attention, memory, and memory-guided orienting change across the menstrual cycle

      2022, Physiology and Behavior
      Citation Excerpt :

      Women's preferences and behaviors could change by reproductive goals in the late FP, including face preference [55–58] and reward perception [59, 60]. The mechanism of mating and face preference in the FP may be attributed to the activation of the reward-network system [59–62]. Masculinized features are more rewarding for women in the FP, so they choose masculine faces over feminine ones [61, 62].

    • The role of ovarian steroids in affective disorders

      2018, Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences
    • Sexual Motivation

      2015, International Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences: Second Edition
    • The influence of steroid sex hormones on the cognitive and emotional processing of visual stimuli in humans

      2013, Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology
      Citation Excerpt :

      Women have been found to show increased medial orbitofrontal cortex, an area involved in reward, activation in response to male faces in the follicular phase of the cycle compared to the luteal phase (Rupp et al., 2009a,b) and women show increased activation in several brain regions toward masculine versus feminine faces in the follicular phase that are suggestive that women find masculinized faces more rewarding. ( Rupp et al., 2009a,b). Although many studies have now demonstrated that women’s preferences for the body odours of symmetric men are enhanced around ovulation (reviewed in Gangestad and Thornhill, 2008), evidence for cyclic shifts in women’s preferences for symmetry in men’s faces is equivocal.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text