eLetters

1526 e-Letters

  • Unpublished methods and results in "Screening for carbon monoxide exposure..."

    ****Please note that BMJ Open received this response on 10 September 2018, but it was not published until 14 January 2019***

    We note the letter from Mr Donnay accusing us of committing research fraud in our 2012 paper [1].
    First, Mr Donnay states that we incorrectly used both arterial and venous COHb. It is clear from the published human literature that there is no significant clinical difference between arterial and venous COHb levels after 10-15 minutes of exposure due to admixture [2-12], although I acknowledge that this message has not got through to some front-line clinicians [13].
    His second point is more pertinent. We became aware of some discrepancies between the non-invasive and venous results. We subsequently became aware of concerns raised by other researchers about the Rad-57 device [14] so we did analyse the data separately in the report to the Department of Health. However, the separate analyses did not alter the fundamental conclusion of our study that a proportion of patients do present to Emergency Departments with higher than expected COHb levels and our hope to raise awareness of possible CO exposure in these people. We stressed that the figures obtained should not be extrapolated more widely and we were careful to avoid confusing higher than expected COHb levels with exposure to non-smoking exogenous sources of CO. We also discussed in depth the limitations of COHb as a biomarker.
    On the note of patient safety our definition of...

    Show More
  • False and misleading claims in reply of Clarke et al

    I am writing to correct false and misleading claims in the reply of Dr. Clarke, two of his co-authors, and Ms. Zamani to my letter of 25 August 2018 documenting evidence of scientific misconduct in their 2012 study of carbon monoxide (CO) poisoning (1).

    For the record, my original letter did not accuse them of “committing research fraud” as they claim. I was open to the possibility that all their misconduct—including not fully reporting their ethics committee approvals, methods, and results—may have been inadvertent or unwitting.

    But this seems less likely in light of their reply, which does not address any of my specific concerns while making new false and misleading claims. They say I allege that they “incorrectly used both arterial and venous COHb,” but my complaint was that they incorrectly used one to validate the other. I assumed this was because they mistook the arterial Rad57 measure for venous. However from their reply it appears they recognized the difference but simply ignored it, based on their mistaken belief that “there is no significant clinical difference [emphasis added] between arterial and venous COHb levels after 10-15 minutes of exposure due to admixture [2-12].”

    In fact, 8 of the 11 studies they cite for this reported just the opposite, finding both clinically and statistically significant differences, including the only study that, like Clarke et al, compared Rad57 measures with venous COHb (12). Only three reported...

    Show More
  • The state of affairs in United Kingdom

    After reading the study thoroughly it comes as a surprise that patients would be actually assessing about there treating physicians and surgeon's knowledge and competence also on basis of there attire rather than handling of affairs, discussions and discourse. The high percentage of preference for White Coats was also indeed refreshing to see in US. Even though in United Kingdom, NHS has embraced the concept of bare elbow for many years now and almost completely let go of the white coat culture, which once used to be a sense of identification and source of pride for doctors everywhere. I believe if such a study is carried out across trusts in United Kingdom the results would be convincingly different. Especially because use of Caucasian models would result in significant bias for the study since NHS is multicultural and not just consisting of Caucasian doctors.. It would be interesting if the same study model is repeated with doctors representing the BAME(Black Asian Minority Ethnicity) as patients preferences regarding there attire would be a stark difference from Caucasian fellow doctors. Also effect of attire would have much greater influence on patients perceiving there doctor as being knowledgeable or not.

  • Biased Study and Misrepresentation of Actual Rates of Plagiarism in African Medical Journals

    We write to express our concern about the prevalence estimate of plagiarism in African medical journals in the study reported by Rohwer et al.(1) The authors’ finding that 63% of African medical journal articles are plagiarized to some degree is a gross overestimate.

    The study definitions of “some,” “moderate,” and “extensive” plagiarism are unvalidated and, as the authors admit in the fourth paragraph of their Discussion section, lack inter-rater reliability and precision. Articles were classified as having “some” plagiarism if there were as few as 1-2 sentences that included identical words or sentences in another article by different authors even if the sentences were properly referenced. Numerous publishing organizations, including the Council of Science Editors,(2) the World Association of Medical Editors,(3) and the US Office of Research Integrity,(4) reserve the use of plagiarism for instances when another’s words are used without proper credit or attribution. The authors developed their definition based on suggestions from the Committee on Publication Ethics (COPE), yet even COPE’s Flowchart for managing suspected plagiarism in a submitted manuscript defines plagiarism as “unattributed use of large portions of text and/or data.”(5)

    In fairness to the African journals implicated in the study, we request the authors go back to their data, identify all instances in which identical wording with formal source citations were defined as plagiarism, recalcu...

    Show More
  • Do not assume homogeneity of experience

    I worked on emergency ambulances in London from 1974 to 1978. I attended several bomb calls during the IRA campaign and it was terrifying to sit in a fibreglass ambulance knowing that there was a real risk of being right next to a car bomb that might be detonated with the intention of harming the emergency services. I was 19 at the time. Looking back, with my current knowledge, I can see that the experience was quite harmful for me. In addition to the existential threat there was also the fear of failing to perform and letting people down and being seen to be a coward. This also took its toll.

    Other things also took a toll and they were not so obvious. Dealing with sick elderly patients was incredibly depressing. I came to fear growing old, because the only old people that I was meeting were ill and depressed and really not enjoying life. I saw so many old people living sad lives in grinding poverty.

    Again, with maturity, this seems totally absurd, but as a 19 year old with no experience of healthy older people (my grandparents were all dead) ambulance work made me very sad. I was surrounded by other ambulance workers who I think tended to be able to cope by having sociopathic tendencies. A very toxic milieu!

    My point here is that when I compared notes with ambulance workers with similar seniority in more rural settings and away from the IRA bombs, their experiences were entirely different. They attended the occasional serious RTC, but mos...

    Show More
  • Social predictors of A&E attendance

    This is a useful study. May I suggest that the Output Area Classification (OAC) offers similar insights based on social groupings. Several studies have explored the use of the OAC relating to A&E attendance and Critical Care admission [1-3]. I suspect that your data is amenable to further helpful analysis using the OAC.

    1. Beeknoo N, Jones R. Factors influencing A&E attendance, admissions and waiting times at two London hospitals. British Journal of Medicine and Medical Research 2016; 17(10): 1-29. doi : 10.9734/BJMMR/2016/28783

    2. Beeknoo N, Jones R. Using Social Groups to Locate Areas with High Emergency Department Attendance, Subsequent Inpatient Admission and Need for Critical Care. British Journal of Medicine and Medical Research 2016; 18(6): 1-23. doi: 10.9734/BJMMR/2016/29208

    3. Beeknoo N, Jones R. Using social groups to locate areas of high utilization of critical care. BBritish Journal of Healthcare Management 2016; 22(11): 551-560.

  • Use of telephone first approach in general practice

    We have read with interest the article by Ball et al (2018) recently published in BMJ Open and note that they refer to our letter (Pereira Gray and Wilkie, 2017) which listed disadvantages of this system from the point of view of patients and where we reported the opposition of the National Association of Patient Participation (N.A.P.P.) to this system.

    We agree with the authors that ‘telephone first’ system is a “fundamental” change in the provision of general practice.
    The authors refer in detail to the independent review of this system Newbould et al (2017), which was published on the BMJ in 2017 and in which three of the authors were the same. We are puzzled by the way they summarise it.

    Authors’ statements in 2018 can be contrasted with the published results in Newbould et al 2017. For example, Ball et al write “No evidence of reduced secondary care costs.” Whereas Newbould et al wrote “we found a significant increase of cost of admissions… leading to an estimated overall increase in secondary care costs of £11,766 per 10,000 patients (i.e. over £1 per patient registered in the general practices).

    Secondly, Ball et al write “Little overall improvement in patient satisfaction as expressed in patient surveys” when Newbould et al reported:
    1.GP communication composite-Significant fall (P<0.001)
    2. Would you recommend your surgery?- Significant fall (P<0.001) 3.Seeing preferred GP- Significant fall (P<0.035)

    Why h...

    Show More
  • Response to letter from Alasdair Philips

    We recently reported on brain tumour incidence time trends in 20 to 59 year old Australians, from 1982 to 2013, and analysed these in terms of mobile phone usage patterns and diagnostic improvements over that interval1. This was designed to determine whether claims that mobile phone use causes brain tumours, are consistent with the pattern of brain tumour incidence in Australia, and in particular to compare such incidence patterns with the results of the multinational Interphone case control study2. In summary, we reported that: 1/ Overall brain tumour incidence rates did not change over time; 2/ Increased glioblastoma incidence was seen during intervals that coincided with improvements in diagnostic technologies (CT, MRI); 3/ Decreased incidence of ‘unspecified’ tumours was seen during the same intervals; and 4/ No evidence of increased tumour incidence (including glioblastoma) related to mobile phone use was found (based on incidence rates seen during the period of substantial mobile phone use and on modelling using a range of hypothetical relative risks and latency periods).

    Philips submitted a Letter to the Editor3 of BMJ Open, where he purports to show that there are ‘significant flaws and unjustifiable conclusions’ in the above paper. Although he may firmly hold this view, his letter does not provide any evidence of this, and we strongly disagree with his statement. We have addressed the substance of his letter below to hopefully obviate potential misunderstan...

    Show More
  • Editor's Note

    This is a note to indicate that BMJ Open is still looking into the disagreement between Albert Donnay and the authors of the paper.

    We will post further updates on the article in due course.

  • Research Assistant - Error correction.

    Page 8/9; Acknowledgements.
    The spelling of research assistant Rohan Navani was misspelt as Rohan Navari.

Pages