Article Text

Laterality, hand control and scholastic performance: a British birth cohort study
  1. Tabita Björk1,2,
  2. Ole Brus3,
  3. Walter Osika4,
  4. Scott Montgomery3,5,6,7
  1. 1Psychiatric Research Centre, Örebro County Council, Örebro, Sweden
  2. 2Department of Clinical Neuroscience, Division of Psychiatry, Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm, Sweden
  3. 3Clinical Epidemiology and Biostatistics Unit, Örebro University Hospital, Örebro, Sweden
  4. 4Stress Research Institute, Stockholm University, Stockholm, Sweden
  5. 5School of Health and Medical Sciences, Örebro University, Örebro, Sweden
  6. 6Clinical Epidemiology Unit, Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm, Sweden
  7. 7Department of Primary Care and Public Health, Charing Cross Hospital, Imperial College, London, UK
  1. Correspondence to Dr Tabita Björk; tabita.bjork{at}orebroll.se

Abstract

Objectives To use simple measures of laterality and hand control that can identify a greater risk of poorer scholastic ability, potentially signalling suboptimal hemispheric lateralisation.

Design Analysis of material from a birth cohort study.

Setting Members of the National Child Development Study, a British birth cohort study following people born in 1958.

Participants 10 612 children who undertook tests at age 11 years.

Primary outcome measures Teacher-administered tests of non-verbal general ability, verbal general ability, reading comprehension and mathematics.

Results Linear regression produced associations (and 95% CIs) with tests of verbal general ability, non-verbal general ability, reading comprehension and mathematics scores for the lowest third (compared with highest) of a left-hand control test involving picking up matches of −1.21 (−1.73 to −0.68; p<0.001), −0.72 (−1.14 to −0.29; p=0.001), −0.70 (−1.06 to −0.35; p<0.001) and −1.32 (−1.90 to −0.73; p<0.001). Among those in the lowest third of the right-hand control test score, mixed-handedness compared with right-handedness was associated with poorer scholastic performance, with regression coefficients (and 95% CIs; p values) of 1.90 (−3.01 to −0.80; p=0.001), −1.25 (−2.15 to −0.35; p=0.007), −1.28 (2.04 to −0.53; p=0.001) and −1.33 (−2.53 to −0.13; p=0.030). The estimates are for a point change in the scholastic test scores, after adjustment for sex, left-hand motor function and social class. Statistically significant associations with mixed-handedness were only observed for the lowest third of right-hand motor function.

Conclusions Measures involving poorer left-hand motor function may represent useful markers of reduced cognitive function possibly reflecting suboptimal hemispheric lateralisation. Crude measures of laterality such as reported non-right-handedness may be more useful for research when combined with measures of motor function.

This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution Non-commercial License, which permits use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited, the use is non commercial and is otherwise in compliance with the license. See: http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/2.0/ and http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/2.0/legalcode.

Statistics from Altmetric.com

Request Permissions

If you wish to reuse any or all of this article please use the link below which will take you to the Copyright Clearance Center’s RightsLink service. You will be able to get a quick price and instant permission to reuse the content in many different ways.

Article summary

Article focus

  • Some patterns of laterality and hand use can signal atypical brain development before birth relevant to problems with language development and scholastic performance. Measures of left- and right-hand control were investigated as markers of cognitive function and were combined with a crude measure of laterality in an attempt to improve its usefulness as a marker of underlying problems relevant to lateralisation and impaired scholastic performance.

Key messages

  • Poorer results from tests of hand control, particularly for the left side, are associated with lower scholastic test scores.

  • Combining crude measures of laterality with hand control test results may improve their usefulness in research to identify children with potentially suboptimal neurological development.

Strengths and limitations of this study

  • The study benefited from use of a large cohort of children representative of the British general population.

  • The study did not identify the neurological processes responsible, or their causes, only potential markers for use in research.

Introduction

Non-right-handedness is linked with a raised risk of scholastic and language difficulties, as well as mental health or behavioural problems such as attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD),1 2 and suboptimal hemispheric lateralisation is one of the factors thought to explain these associations.3–5 Research using a longitudinal birth cohort study in Finland6 demonstrated that the most notable associations with impaired scholastic ability are among those who are mixed-handed (ambidextrous) rather than left-handed. Unsophisticated measures such as reported right-handedness, left-handedness and mixed-handedness, as used in the Finnish study,6 are imprecise markers of subtle developmental deficits and may therefore be of limited use in research. This paper is concerned with examining simple measures of laterality and hand control including measures of motor function, which may signal developmental deficits relevant to scholastic ability.

Motor function is associated with ability in both reading and mathematics7–10 and thus a useful indicator of relevant general neurological function. We used a measure of motor function, involving tasks for the left and right hands. The aims of using the motor test scores were to identify children among whom non-right-handedness is a better indicator of poorer scholastic ability and also to examine whether the motor test results themselves provide more useful information than an unsophisticated classification of laterality (left-, right- and mixed-handed where no hand preference was specified).

A British general population-based birth cohort study11 provided information on laterality, and two tests of motor function for both left and right hand were administered by a doctor at age 11 years: time to pick up 20 matches and number of boxes ticked within a time limit. We chose to use the picking up matches test, as ticking boxes involves writing skills, which could be more influenced by cultural norms for hand preference,12 and therefore, we hypothesised potentially less precise as a less dominant hand may be used more, thus improving performance. For outcome measures of cognitive or scholastic ability, we used four tests conducted at age 11 years. The main analysis was stratified by level of right-hand motor function (in equal thirds of the distribution). The right hand was chosen as it tends to be the dominant hand.

Methods

Research setting and study design

A total of 10 612 cohort members from The National Child Development Study (NCDS)11 were included in the analyses. NCDS is following everyone born between 3 and 9 March 1958 and living in Great Britain, with data collection sweeps at various ages.13 The study originally comprised approximately 17 000 births, but the subsequent exclusion of Northern Ireland, death, emigration and other causes of attrition reduced the sample size, although the cohort has remained broadly representative of the target population.14

Measures

Laterality and hand control

When the children were aged 7 years, mothers were asked if they thought their child was right-handed, left-handed, ambidextrous (mixed handed) or don't know. Local authority medical officers administered a series of functional assessments. The chosen test of motor function was undertaken at age 11 years and recorded the time in seconds that it took to pick up 20 matches (maximum 99 s), separately for left and right hand, with a higher score indicating poorer performance.

Scholastic ability

Teachers administered tests of cognition and scholastic ability at school when the children were 11 years old. These tests were for verbal general ability (score range 0–40), non-verbal general ability (0–40), reading comprehension (0–35) and mathematics (0–40). The tests were constructed specifically for use in NCDS by the National Foundation for Educational Research in England and Wales.11 15

Other measures

At age 11 years, local authority medical officers conducted a medical examination and record review, which identified children with a ‘general motor handicap’ with categories: ‘none’, ‘condition present but no handicap’, ‘slight handicap’, ‘moderate handicap’ or ‘severe handicap’. Social class (Registrar General) based on the father's occupation was categorised as I, II, III non-manual, III manual, IV non-manual, IV manual, V, no father present and ‘not assigned’ (where there was insufficient information).

Inclusion and exclusion criteria

Information on laterality and the picking up matches test score was available for 11 489 children. Of these, 37 reported uncertain laterality and were excluded. A further 129 were excluded due to missing data on parental social class. From among those remaining, 10 657 completed at least one scholastic ability test, but 45 were excluded due to an observed or diagnosed condition or disability relevant to motor function. Data for 10 612 children were available for the tests of verbal general ability, non-verbal general ability and reading comprehension. Data for 10 608 children were available for the mathematics test.

Statistical analysis

Means and cross-tabulation describe the study population, and this analysis was stratified by laterality. The scores for picking up matches (the marker of motor control) for left and right hands were divided into equal thirds of their distributions.

Linear regression models used test scores as dependent variables: verbal general ability, non-verbal general ability, reading comprehension and mathematics. Analyses were externally stratified by right-hand motor function test thirds (separate analyses for each third). Further analysis combined the two highest right-hand motor function thirds to ensure that statistical significance in the lowest function third could not be explained by greater statistical power. Variables included in the analyses were laterality, left-hand motor test score (in thirds), sex and social class. The measures were modelled as series of binary dummy variables, first examined separately and then with mutual simultaneous adjustment.

Interaction testing assessed whether stratification by right-hand motor function modified the associations of laterality with the tests of scholastic ability. Interaction terms (and main effects) were included in non-stratified linear regression models with the measures of scholastic ability as dependent variables. Similar interaction testing examined the combination of left- and right-hand motor function (in thirds of their distributions due to non-linearity). PASW V.18 software was used.

Ethics

Specific ethical permission was not required for this secondary analysis of anonymised data.

Results

Table 1 shows the population characteristics, presented in separate columns for right-, left- and mixed-handedness. Non-right-handedness was associated with somewhat lower average test scores for general ability (both verbal and non-verbal), reading comprehension and mathematics, with little difference between left- and mixed-handedness.

Table 1

Characteristics of the study population divided by hand laterality

As expected, left-handed cohort members were more often in the highest third for the left-hand motor function test and the lowest third for the right-hand test. Boys predominate among the left- or mixed-handed. Non-right-handedness is somewhat associated with lower parental social class.

In tables 2–5, results are stratified into thirds of the right-hand motor function test (picking up matches), where the top section of the tables is for those with the poorest performance. The number of subjects in each stratum and laterality group can be ascertained from table 1.

Table 2

Verbal general ability test and laterality, stratified by right-hand motor score

Table 3

Non-verbal general ability test and laterality, stratified by right-hand motor score

Table 4

Reading comprehension test and laterality, stratified by right-hand motor score

Table 5

Mathematics test and laterality, stratified by right-hand motor score

Verbal general ability

Among those in the lowest right-hand motor function group, left- and particularly mixed-handedness, compared with right-handedness, are statistically significantly and independently associated with lower verbal general ability scores (table 2). Statistically significant associations for laterality were not observed in the two higher thirds for right-hand motor function. Interaction testing provided evidence of effect modification by right-hand motor function for mixed-handedness (p=0.039), but not left-handedness (p=0.980). Lower left-hand motor function test scores were statistically significantly associated with lower verbal general ability scores in the lowest right-hand stratum, independent of the potential confounding factors (although interaction testing was not significant). A borderline statistically significantly reduced score for low left-hand function was also observed in the highest right hand function stratum.

Non-verbal general ability

Among those in the lowest right-hand motor function stratum, lower non-verbal general ability scores were statistically significantly and independently associated with left- and particularly mixed-handedness compared with right-handedness (table 3). Non-right-handedness was not statistically significantly associated with the verbal general ability scores in the two higher right-hand motor function strata. Interaction testing provided evidence of effect modification by right-hand motor function for mixed-handedness (p=0.012), but not left-handedness (p=0.786).

Lower left-hand motor function test scores were statistically significantly associated with lower verbal general ability scores in the lowest right-hand performance third, independent of the potential confounding factors. Interaction testing suggests effect modification for the combination of low left-hand function with low right function (p=0.007).

Reading comprehension

Among those in the lowest right-hand motor function stratum, lower reading comprehension scores were associated with mixed-handedness compared with right-handedness (table 4). After adjustment, the associations with both left- and mixed-handedness were statistically significant. Non-right-handedness was not statistically significantly associated with the reading comprehension scores in the two higher right-hand performance strata. Interaction testing provided evidence of effect modification by right-hand motor function for mixed-handedness (p<0.001), but not left-handedness (p=0.598).

Lower left-hand motor function test scores were statistically significantly associated with lower reading comprehension scores in the lowest and highest right-hand performance thirds, independent of the potential confounding factors (although interaction testing was not statistically significant).

Mathematics

Among those in the lowest right-hand motor function stratum, lower test scores were associated with left- and particularly mixed-handedness compared with right-handedness (table 5).

These associations were statistically significant and independent of the potential confounding factors. Non-right-handedness was not statistically significantly associated with the mathematics test scores in the two higher right-hand performance strata. Interaction testing did not provide strong evidence of effect modification by right-hand motor function for mixed-handedness (p=0.195) or left-handedness (p=0.810).

Lower left-hand motor function test scores were statistically significantly associated with lower mathematics test scores in the lowest and highest right-hand performance thirds, independent of the potential confounding factors. Interaction testing did not identify effect modification (p>0.05).

In further analyses, the two higher right-hand motor function score thirds were combined to ensure that the statistical significance of associations of laterality with the scholastic test scores in the lowest right-hand motor function stratum were not due to greater statistical power. Neither left- nor mixed-handedness is statistically significantly associated with any of the scholastic tests in the combined top two thirds for right-hand motor function (data not shown). There were no notable gradients in the association of laterality with the scholastic test scores across the two higher right-hand motor function strata. Although the average scholastic test scores tended to be lower among those who were mixed-handed than for left-handedness, no statistically significant differences were observed when mixed-handedness was compared with left-handedness.

Non-stratified analysis

Table 6 shows associations with each of the four scholastic tests for laterality, right-hand motor function and left-hand motor function test scores. Right-handedness and the highest third of the motor function tests were used as reference categories. In the adjusted models, non-right-handedness was associated with statistically significantly poorer scholastic ability, except for the reading comprehension test. It is notable that cohort members with left-hand motor scores in lowest third had lower average scholastic test scores than those with parentally reported non-right-handed laterality. Poorer left-hand function was associated with lower average scholastic scores than poorer right-hand function, except for non-verbal general ability where low right-hand function was associated with somewhat lower scores.

Table 6

Non-stratified analysis of associations with each of the four scholastic tests, for laterality and the hand motor function tests

The online appendix presents the results of further analyses showing similar associations, where the motor function test involved ticking boxes rather than picking up matches.

Discussion

Although the a priori hypothesis for this study involved the combination of lack of reported hand preference (described as ‘mixed-handedness’) with a measure of right-hand motor function, the measures of left- and right-hand motor function themselves may have produced more informative results. Poorer motor function in both hands, but particularly the left, was associated with poorer scholastic ability. The combination of poor left-hand motor function with either low or high right-hand function was associated with lower average test scores. Non-right-handedness alone was not notably associated with poorer scholastic ability; only the combination of non-right-handedness and lower right hand motor function (the lowest third) was consistently associated with poor scholastic ability. Interaction testing indicated effect modification for the specific combination of no reported hand preference and poorer motor performance.

A previous study demonstrated an association between ‘mixed-handedness’ (lack of reported hand preference) and poorer scholastic performance, where laterality was reported by parents using a single-item question.6 For comparability, we also used parent-reported laterality but combined this with left- and right-hand motor function tests. Associations of motor function16–18 and laterality6–10 with intelligence and scholastic ability have been demonstrated previously, but this combination of motor function with this measure of handedness in the general population is more novel. Studies of laterality often use a series of questions for more precise classification,19–21 rather than the single question used by this study which limits accuracy significantly. A more elaborate approach also allows grading of strength of left- or right-handedness. A lack of reported hand preference may reflect uncertainty about laterality,19 so a more precise instrument should reduce the misclassification that is likely to have existed in our study. For this reason, our ‘mixed-handed’ group may be heterogeneous, and the results using parent-reported handedness should be interpreted with caution. Despite this imprecision, interpretability of the results may have been increased by our use of more objective left- and right-hand motor function measures. Although imprecise, the laterality measure used here can be compared with other studies that have used a similar simple measure. Non-right-handedness among the lowest right-hand motor function group may help to identify a small subgroup at greater risk of having suboptimal hemispheric lateralisation.

Motor function is associated with cognitive ability,7–10 and a striking feature of our findings is the magnitude of the association of poor left hand motor function with worse scholastic test scores. Interestingly, the combination of low left-hand function with either low or high right-hand function was associated with lower scholastic test scores. This is consistent with Marian Annett's right shift theory of handedness and cerebral dominance, where hypothetical genetic factors produce a continuum of hemispheric asymmetry.22 A lack of asymmetry may be detrimental for some aspects of hemispheric specialisation, but excessive asymmetry (as signalled by high right- and low left-hand motor function) may also limit some functions relevant to cognition and behaviour.22 Strong right-handedness has been linked with poorer cognitive function, including for episodic memory.23–25

In addition to stratifying by right hand motor function and adjusting for left hand motor function, we adjusted for several other measures to ensure that the results were not due to confounding. We adjusted for sex as it is associated with laterality, motor control and scholastic ability.26 27 We also included parental social class to improve precision, as there are social influences on learning and problem solving.28 To ensure that the results are not influenced by a small group of children with disabilities, children with any diagnosed motor disabilities were excluded from the analysis. We did not adjust for developmental factors, as the study was designed to examine measures of handedness, rather than identify causes.

While this study cannot identify the neurological mechanisms underlying associations between laterality or hand control and scholastic performance, suboptimal hemispheric lateralisation is one possible component of the explanation. Task specialisation between the left and right hemispheres can be associated with cerebral asymmetry in anatomy, neurotransmitter expression and inter-hemispheric signalling.29–31 A high degree of lateralisation in function may result in, or be influenced by, higher level cognitive function.32 Abnormalities in cerebral asymmetry and inter-hemispheric signalling have been associated with developmental and behavioural problems such as ADHD,4 so related difficulties may help explain our findings. Boys appear to be particularly susceptible to in utero exposures relevant to suboptimal lateralisation, as premature boys are at greater risk of non-right-handedness and behavioural disorders like ADHD.33 34 A consistent finding is that more of the boys in this study were non-right-handed. Functional cortical asymmetry can vary with age,30 indicating that later life exposures may influence whether suboptimal lateralisation persists.

Potential limitations of this study include the imprecise laterality measure reported by mothers, particularly as the ‘mixed-handed’ category reflects no reported hand preference rather than a more structured assessment of whether an individual is ambidextrous. Division of the motor function scores into equal thirds may not be optimal but chosen on an a priori basis to demonstrate a constant pattern with the minimum number of groups. To ensure that differences in statistical power did not account for the findings, the two higher function thirds were combined and statistical significant association for mixed handedness were still only observed in the lowest function third. A limitation of the stratified design is that it could underestimate the overall magnitude of associations with scholastic test scores. The cohort remained largely representative of the general population, but loss of the most disadvantaged children35 may have limited participation of some children with the poorest scholastic performance. The tests used here cannot identify causes or underlying mechanisms and these may be heterogeneous.

Conclusions

Simple objective tests of left- and right-hand motor function may be used to produce more useful markers of neurological characteristics relevant to scholastic performance than crude reports of left-handedness, right-handedness or where no hand preference is reported. If a crude measure of laterality is all that is available, combining it with a measure of objective motor function may improve its utility. The notably worse scholastic scores associated with poor left-hand function suggest that this may be a useful measure alone or in combination with others.

References

Supplementary materials

  • Supplementary Data

    This web only file has been produced by the BMJ Publishing Group from an electronic file supplied by the author(s) and has not been edited for content.

    Files in this Data Supplement:

Footnotes

  • To cite: Björk T, Brus O, Osika W, et al. Laterality, hand control and scholastic performance: a British birth cohort study. BMJ Open 2012;2:e000314. doi:10.1136/bmjopen-2011-000314

  • Contributors Dr TB was involved in drafting the manuscript and developing the analytic strategy. Mr OB was responsible for conducting the statistical analysis and produced the tables. Dr WO provided input on aspects of neurodevelopment. Professor SM devised the hypothesis and study design and was involved in drafting the manuscript. All the authors contributed to critical revision of the manuscript.

  • Funding This study received funding from the Örebro County Council Research Committee, Sweden, and from Economic and Social Research Council grant RES 596-28-0001 to the International Centre for Life Course Studies.

  • Competing interests None.

  • Patient consent Obtained.

  • Provenance and peer review Not commissioned; externally peer reviewed.

  • Data sharing statement All data used for this research were from the National Child development Study, available through the UK Data Archive (http://www.data-archive.ac.uk/).